CHAPTERVI.EMOTIONAL AND IMITATIVE LANGUAGE (continued).
Imitative Words—Human actions named from sound—Animals’ names from cries, &c.—Musical Instruments—Sounds reproduced—Words modified to adapt sound to sense—Reduplication—Graduation of vowels to express distance and difference—Children’s Language—Sound-words as related to Sense-words—Language an original product of the lower Culture.
From the earliest times of language to our own day, it is unlikely that men ever quite ceased to be conscious that some of their words were derived from imitation of the common sounds heard about them. In our own modern English, for instance, results of such imitation are evident; fliesbuzz, beeshum, snakeshiss, a cracker or a bottle of ginger-beerpops, a cannon or a bitternbooms. In the words for animals and for musical instruments in the various languages of the world, the imitation of their cries and tones is often to be plainly heard, as in the names of thehoopoe, theai-aisloth, thekakaparrot, the Easterntomtom, which is a drum, the Africanulule, which is a flute, the Siamesekhong-bong, which is a wooden harmonicon, and in like manner through a host of other words. But these evident cases are far from representing the whole effects of imitation on the growth of language. They form, indeed, the easy entrance to a philological region, which becomes less penetrable the farther it is explored.
The operations of which we see the results before us in the actual languages of the world seem to have been somewhat as follows. Men have imitated their own emotional utterances or interjections, the cries of animals, the tones ofmusical instruments, the sounds of shouting, howling, stamping, breaking, tearing, scraping, with others which are all day coming to their ears, and out of these imitations many current words indisputably have their source. But these words, as we find them in use, differ often widely, often beyond all recognition, from the original sounds they sprang from. In the first place, man’s voice can only make a very rude copy of most sounds his ear receives; his possible vowels are very limited in their range compared with natural tones, and his possible consonants still more helpless as a means of imitating natural noises. Moreover, his voice is only allowed to use a part even of this imperfect imitative power, seeing that each language for its own convenience restricts it to a small number of set vowels and consonants, to which the imitative sounds have to conform, thus becoming conventionalized into articulate words with further loss of imitative accuracy. No class of words have a more perfect imitative origin than those which simply profess to be vocal imitations of sound. How ordinary alphabets to some extent succeed and to some extent fail in writing down these sounds may be judged from a few examples. Thus, the Australian imitation of a spear or bullet striking is given astoop; to the Zulu, when a calabash is beaten, it saysboo; the Karens hear the flitting ghosts of the dead call in the wailing voice of the wind,re, re, ro, ro; the old traveller, Pietro della Valle, tells how the Shah of Persia sneered at Timur and his Tartars, with their arrows that wentter ter; certain Buddhist heretics maintained that water is alive, because when it boils it sayschichitá, chitichita, a symptom of vitality which occasioned much theological controversy as to drinking cold and warm water. Lastly, sound-words taken up into the general inventory of a language have to follow its organic changes, and in the course of phonetic transition, combination, decay, and mutilation, to lose ever more and more their original shape. To take a single example, the Frenchhuer‘to shout’ (Welshhwa) may be a perfect imitative verb; yet when it passes into modernEnglishhueand cry, our changed pronunciation of the vowel destroys all imitation of the call. Now to the language-makers all this was of little account. They merely wanted recognized words to express recognized thought, and no doubt arrived by repeated trials at systems which were found practically to answer this purpose. But to the modern philologist, who is attempting to work out the converse of the problem, and to follow backward the course of words to original imitative sound, the difficulty is most embarrassing. It is not only that thousands of words really derived from such imitation may now by successive change have lost all safe traces of their history; such mere deficiency of knowledge is only a minor evil. What is far worse is that the way is thrown open to an unlimited number of false solutions, which yet look on the face of them fully as like truth as others which we know historically to be true. One thing is clear, that it is of no use to resort to violent means, to rush in among the words of language, explaining them away right and left as derived each from some remote application of an imitative noise. The advocate of the Imitative Theory who attempts this, trusting in his own powers of discernment, has indeed taken in hand a perilous task, for, in fact, of all judges of the question at issue, he has nourished and trained himself up to become the very worst. His imagination is ever suggesting to him what his judgment would like to find true; like a witness answering the questions of the counsel on his own side, he answers in good faith, but with what bias we all know. It was thus with De Brosses, to whom this department of philology owes so much. It is nothing to say that he had a keen ear for the voice of Nature; she must have positively talked to him in alphabetic language, for he could hear the sound of hollowness in theskofσκάπτω‘to dig,’ of hardness in thecalofcallosity, the noise of insertion of a body between two others in thetroftrans,intra. In enquiries so liable to misleading fancy, no pains should be spared in securing impartial testimony, and it fortunatelyhappens that there are available sources of such evidence, which, when thoroughly worked, will give to the theory of imitative words as near an approach to accuracy as has been attained to in any other wide philological problem. By comparing a number of languages, widely apart in their general system and materials, and whose agreement as to the words in question can only be accounted for by similar formation of words from similar suggestion of sound, we obtain groups of words whose imitative character is indisputable. The groups here considered consist in general of imitative words of the simpler kind, those directly connected with the special sound they are taken from, but their examination to some extent admits of words being brought in, where the connexion of the idea expressed with the sound imitated is more remote. This, lastly, opens the far wider and more difficult problem, how far imitation of sounds is the primary cause of the great mass of words in the vocabularies of the world, between whose sound and sense no direct connexion appears.
Words which express human actions accompanied with sound form a very large and intelligible class. In remote and most different languages, we find such forms aspu,puf,bu,buf,fu,fuf, in use with the meaning ofpuffing,fuffing; or blowing; Malaypuput; Tonganbuhi; Maoripupui; Australianbobun,bwa-bun; Gallabufa,afufa; Zulufuta,punga,pupuza(fu,pu, used as expressive particles); Quichépuba; Quichuapuhuni; Tupiypeû; Finnishpuhkia; Hebrewpuach; Danishpuste; Lithuanianpúciu; and in numbers of other languages;[283]here, grammatical adjuncts apart, the significant force lies in the imitative syllable. Savages have named the European musket when they saw it, by the soundpu, describing not the report, but the puff of smoke issuing from the muzzle. The Society Islanders supposed at first that the white men blew through the barrel of the gun, and they called it accordinglypupuhi, from the verbpuhitoblow, while the New Zealanders more simply called it apu. So the Amaxosa of South Africa call itumpu, from the imitative soundpu!The Chinook Jargon of North-West America uses the phrasemamook poo(makepoo) for a verb ‘to shoot,’ and a six-chambered revolver is calledtohum poo,i.e., a ‘six-poo.’ When a European uses the wordpuffto denote the discharge of a gun, he is merely referring to the smoke blown out, as he would speak of apuffof wind, or even a powder-puffor apuff-ball; and when a pistol is called in colloquial German apuffer, the meaning of the word matches that used for it in French Argot, a ‘soufflant.’ It has often been supposed that thepuffimitates the actual sound, thebangof the gun, and this has been brought forward to show by what extremely different words one and the same sound may be imitated, but this is a mistake.[284]These derivations of the name of the gun from the notion of blowing correspond with those which give names to the comparatively noiseless blow-tube of the bird-hunter, called by the Indians of Yucatan apub, in South America by the Chiquitos apucuna, by the Cocamas apuna. Looking into vocabularies of languages which have such verbs ‘to blow,’ it is usual to find with them other words apparently related to them, and expressing more or less distant ideas. Thus Australianpoo-yu,puyu‘smoke;’ Quichuapuhucuni‘to light a fire,’punquini‘to swell,’puyu,puhuyu‘a cloud;’ Maoripuku‘to pant,’puka‘to swell;’ Tupipúpú,pupúre‘to boil;’ Gallabube‘wind,’bubiza‘to cool by blowing;’ Kanuri (rootfu)fungin‘to blow, swell,’furúdu‘a stuffed pad or bolster,’ &c.,bubute‘bellows’ (bubute fungin‘I blow the bellows’); Zulu (dropping the prefixes)puku,pukupu‘frothing, foam,’ whencepukupuku‘an empty frothy fellow,’pupuma‘to bubble, boil,’fu‘a cloud,’fumfu‘blown about like high grass in the wind,’ whencefumfuta‘to be confused, thrown into disorder,’futo‘bellows,’fuba‘the breast, chest,’ then figuratively ‘bosom, conscience.’
The group of words belonging to the closed lips, of whichmum,mumming,mumbleare among the many forms belonging to European languages,[285]are worked out in like manner among the lower races—Veimu mu‘dumb’; Mpongweimamu‘dumb’; Zulumomata(frommoma, ‘a motion with the mouth as in mumbling’) ‘to move the mouth or lips,’mumata‘to close the lips as with a mouthful of water,’mumuta,mumuza‘to eat mouthfuls of corn, &c., with the lips shut;’ Tahitianmamu‘to be silent,’omumu‘to murmur;’ Fijian,nomo,nomo-nomo‘to be silent;’ Chilian,ñomn‘to be silent;’ Quiché,mem‘mute,’ whencememer‘to become mute;’ Quichua,amu‘dumb, silent,’amullini‘to have something in the mouth,’amul-layacuni simicta‘to mutter, to grumble.’ The group represented by Sanskritt’hût’hû‘the sound of spitting,’ Persianthu kerdan(makethu) ‘to spit,’ Greekπτύω, may be compared with Chinookmamook toh,tooh, (maketoh,tooh); Chiliantuvcùtun(maketuv); Tahitiantutua; Gallatwu; Yorubatu. Among the Sanskrit verb-roots, none carries its imitative nature more plainly thankshu‘to sneeze;’ the following analogous forms are from South America:—Chilian,echiun; Quichua,achhini; and from various languages of Brazilian tribes,techa-ai,haitschu,atchian,natschun,aritischune, &c. Another imitative verb is well shown in the Negro-English dialect of Surinam,njam‘to eat’ (pron.nyam),njam-njam‘food’ (‘en hemnjanjamben de sprinkhan nanga boesi-honi’—‘and his meat was locusts and wild honey’). In Australia the imitative verb ‘to eat’ reappears asg’nam-ang. In Africa the Susu language hasnimnim, ‘to taste,’ and a similar formation is observed in the Zulunambita‘to smack the lips after eating or tasting, and thence to be tasteful, to be pleasant to the mind.’ This is an excellent instance of the transition of mere imitative sound to the expression of mental emotion, and it corresponds with the imitative way in which the Yakama language, in speaking of little childrenor pet animals, expresses the verb ‘to love’ asnem-no-sha(to maken’m-n’). In more civilized countries these forms are mostly confined to baby-language. The Chinese child’s word for eating isnam, in English nurseriesnimis noticed as answering the same purpose, and the Swedish dictionary even recognizesnamnam‘a tid-bit.’
As for imitative names of animals derived from their cries or noises, they are to be met with in every language from the Australiantwonk‘frog,’ the Yakamarol-rol‘lark,’ to the Copticeeiō‘ass,’ the Chinesemaou‘cat,’ and the Englishcuckooandpeewit. Their general principle of formation being acknowledged, their further philological interest turns mostly on cases where corresponding words have thus been formed independently in distant regions, and those where the imitative name of the creature, or its habitual sound, passes to express some new idea suggested by its character. The Sanskrit name of thekâkacrow reappears in the name of a similar bird in British Columbia, thekáh-káh; a fly is called by the natives of Australia abumberoo, like Sanskritbambharâli‘fly,’ Greekβομ-βύλιος, and ourbumble-bee. Analogous to the name of thetse-tsefly, the terror of African travellers, isntsintsi, the word for ‘fly’ among the Basutos, which also, by a simple metaphor, serves to express the idea of ‘a parasite.’ Mr.H. W.Bates’s description seems to settle the dispute among naturalists, whether thetoucanhad its name from its cry or not. He speaks of its loud, shrill, yelping cries having ‘a vague resemblance to the syllablestocáno, tocáno, and hence the Indian name of this genus of birds.’ Granting this, we can trace this sound-word into a very new meaning; for it appears that the bird’s monstrous bill has suggested a name for a certain large-nosed tribe of Indians, who are accordingly calledTucanos.[286]The cock,galloquiquiriqui, as the Spanish nursery-language calls him, has a long list of names from various languageswhich in various ways imitate his crowing; in Yoruba he is calledkoklo, in Ibookoko,akoka, in Zulukuku, in Finnishkukko, in Sanskritkukkuta, and so on. He is mentioned in the Zend-Avesta in a very curious way, by a name which elaborately imitates his cry, but which the ancient Persians seem to have held disrespectful to their holy bird, who rouses men from sleep to good thought, word, and work:—
‘The bird who bears the name of Parôdars, O holy Zarathustra;Upon whom evil-speaking men impose the nameKabrkataç.’[287]
‘The bird who bears the name of Parôdars, O holy Zarathustra;Upon whom evil-speaking men impose the nameKabrkataç.’[287]
‘The bird who bears the name of Parôdars, O holy Zarathustra;Upon whom evil-speaking men impose the nameKabrkataç.’[287]
‘The bird who bears the name of Parôdars, O holy Zarathustra;
Upon whom evil-speaking men impose the nameKabrkataç.’[287]
The crowing of the cock (Malaykâluruk,kukuk) serves to mark a point of time, cockcrow. Other words originally derived from such imitation of crowing have passed into other curiously transformed meanings: Old Frenchcocart‘vain;’ modern Frenchcoquet‘strutting like acock,coquetting, acoxcomb;’cocarde‘acockade’ (from its likeness to a cock’s comb); one of the best instances iscoquelicot, a name given for the same reason to the wild poppy, and even more distinctly in Languedoc, wherecacaracámeans both the crowing and the flower. The hen in some languages has a name corresponding to that of the cock, as in Kussakukuduna‘cock,’kukukasi‘hen;’ Ewekoklo-tsu‘cock,’koklo-no‘hen;’ and hercackle(whence she has in Switzerland the name ofgugel,güggel) has passed into language as a term for idle gossip and chatter of women,caquet,caqueter,gackern, much as the noise of a very different creature seems to have given rise not only to its name, Italiancicala, but to a group of words represented bycicalar‘to chirp, chatter, talk sillily.’ Thepigeonis a good example of this kind, both for sound and sense. It is Latinpipio, Italianpippione,piccione,pigione, modern Greekπιπίνιον, Frenchpipion(old),pigeon; its derivation is from the young bird’speep, Latinpipire, Italianpipiare,pigiolare, modern Greekπιπινίζω, to chirp; by an easy metaphor, apigeoncomes to mean ‘a silly young felloweasily caught,’ topigeon‘to cheat,’ Italianpipione‘a silly gull, one that is soon caught and trepanned,’pippionare‘to pigeon, to gull one.’ In an entirely different family of languages, Mr. Wedgwood points out a curiously similar process of derivation; Magyarpipegni,pipelni‘topeepor cheep;’pipe,pipök‘a chicken, gosling;’pipe-ember(chicken-man), ‘a silly young fellow, booby.’[288]The derivation of Greekβοῦς, Latinbos, Welshbu, from the ox’s lowing, orbooingas it is called in the north country, has been much debated. With an excessive desire to make Sanskrit answer as a general Indo-European type, Bopp connected Sanskritgo, old Germanchuo, Englishcow, with these words, on the unusual and forced assumption of a change from guttural to labial.[289]The direct derivation from sound, however, is favoured by other languages, Cochin-Chinesebo, Hottentotbou. The beast may almost answer for himself in the words of that Spanish proverb which remarks that people talk according to their nature:‘Habló elbuey, y dijóbu!’‘The ox spoke, and he saidboo!’
Among musical instruments with imitative names are the following:—theshee-shee-quoi, the mystic rattle of the Red Indian medicine-man, an imitative word which reappears in the Darien Indianshak-shak, theshook-shookof the Arawaks, the Chinookshugh(whenceshugh-opoots, rattletail,i.e., ‘rattlesnake;’)—the drum, calledgangain Haussa,gañgañin the Yoruba country,gungumaby the Gallas, and having its analogue in the Easterngong;—the bell, called in Yakama (N. Amer.)kwa-lal-kwa-lal, in Yalof (W.Afr.)walwal, in Russiankolokol. The sound of the horn is imitated in English nurseries astoot-toot, and this is transferred to express the ‘omnibus’ of which the bugle is the signal: with this nursery word is to be classed thePeruvian name for the ‘shell-trumpet,’pututu, and the Gothicthuthaurn(thut-horn), which is even used in the Gothic Bible for the last trumpet of the day of judgement,—‘In spêdistin thuthaúrna, thuthaúrneith auk jah daúthans ustandand’(ICor.xv.52). How such imitative words, when thoroughly taken up into language, suffer change of pronunciation in which the original sound-meaning is lost, may be seen in the English wordtabor, which we might not recognize as a sound-word at all, did we not notice that it is Frenchtabour, a word which in the formtambourobviously belongs to a group of words for drums, extending from the small rattling Arabictublto the Indiandundhubiand thetombe, the Moqui drum made of a hollowed log. The same group shows the transfer of such imitative words to objects which are like the instrument, but have nothing to do with its sound; few people who talk oftambour-work, and fewer still who speak of a footstool as atabouret, associate these words with the sound of a drum, yet the connexion is clear enough. When these two processes go on together, and a sound-word changes its original sound on the one hand, and transfers its meaning to something else on the other, the result may soon leave philological analysis quite helpless, unless by accident historical evidence is forthcoming. Thus with the English wordpipe. Putting aside the particular pronunciation which we give the word, and referring it back to its mediæval Latin or French sound inpipa,pipe, we have before us an evident imitative name of a musical instrument, derived from a familiar sound used also to represent the chirping of chickens, Latinpipire, English topeep, as in the translation of Isaiahviii.19: ‘Seek ... unto wizards thatpeep, and that mutter.’ The Algonquin Indians appear to have formed from this soundpib(with a grammatical suffix) their name for thepib-e-gwunor native flute. Now just astuba,tubus, ‘a trumpet’ (itself very likely an imitative word) has given a name for any kind oftube, so the wordpipehas been transferred from the musicalinstrument to which it first belonged, and is used to describe tubes of various sorts, gas-pipes, water-pipes, and pipes in general. There is nothing unusual in these transitions of meaning, which are in fact rather the rule than the exception. Thechiboukwas originally a herdsman’s pipe or flute in Central Asia. Thecalumet, popularly ranked with the tomahawk and the mocassin among characteristic Red Indian words, is only the name for a shepherd’s pipe (Latincalamus) in the dialect of Normandy, corresponding with thechalumeauof literary French; for when the early colonists in Canada saw the Indians performing the strange operation of smoking, ‘with a hollow piece of stone or wood like a pipe,’ as Jacques Cartier has it, they merely gave to the native tobacco-pipe the name of the French musical instrument it resembled. Now changes of sound and of sense like this of the English wordpipemust have been in continual operation in hundreds of languages where we have no evidence to follow them by, and where we probably may never obtain such evidence. But what little we do know must compel us to do justice to the imitation of sound as a really existing process, capable of furnishing an indefinitely large supply of words for things and actions which have no necessary connexion at all with that sound. Where the traces of the transfer are lost, the result is a stock of words which are the despair of philologists, but are perhaps none the less fitted for the practical use of men who simply want recognized symbols for recognized ideas.
The claim of the Easterntomtomto have its name from a mere imitation of its sound seems an indisputable one; but when it is noticed in what various languages the beating of a resounding object is expressed by something liketum,tumb,tump,tup, as in Javantumbuk, Coptictmno, ‘to pound in a mortar,’ it becomes evident that the admission involves more than at first sight appears. In Malay,timpa,tampa, is ‘to beat out, hammer, forge;’ in the Chinook Jargontum-tumis ‘the heart,’ and by combining the same soundwith the English word ‘water,’ a name is made for ‘waterfall,’tum-wâta. The Gallas of East Africa declare that a box on the ear seems to them to make a noise liketub, for they call its soundtubdjeda, that is, ‘to saytub.’ In the same language,tumais ‘to beat,’ whencetumtu, ‘a workman, especially one who beats, a smith.’ With the aid of another imitative word,bufa‘to blow,’ the Gallas can construct this wholly imitative sentence,tumtun bufa bufti, ‘the smith blows with bellows,’ as an English child might say, ‘thetumtum puffsthepuffer.’ This imitative sound seems to have obtained a footing among the Aryan verb-roots, as in Sanskrittup,tubh‘to smite,’ while in Greek,tup,tump, has the meaning of ‘to beat, tothump,’ producing for instanceτύμπανον,tympanum, ‘a drum ortomtom.’ Again, the verb tocrackhas become in modern English as thorough a root-word as the language possesses. The mere imitation of the sound of breaking has passed into a verb to break; we speak of acrackedcup or acrackedreputation without a thought of imitation of sound; but we cannot yet use the Germankrachenor Frenchcraquerin this way, for they have not developed in meaning as our word has, but remain in their purely imitative stage. There are two corresponding Sanskrit words for the saw,kra-kara,kra-kacha, that is to say, the ‘kra-maker,kra-crier;’ and it is to be observed that all such terms, which expressly state that they are imitations of sound, are particularly valuable evidence in these enquiries, for whatever doubt there may be as to other words being really derived from imitative sound, there can, of course, be none here. Moreover, there is evidence of the same sound having given rise to imitative words in other families of language, Dahomankra-kra, ‘a watchman’s rattle;’ Grebogrikâ‘a saw;’ Ainochacha‘to saw;’ Malaygraji‘a saw,’karat‘to gnash the teeth,’karot‘to make agratingnoise;’ Coptickhrij‘to gnash the teeth,’khrajrej‘tograte.’ Another form of the imitation is given in the descriptive Galla expressioncacakdjeda,i.e., ‘to saycacak,’ ‘tocrack,krachen.’ With this sound corresponds a whole family of Peruvian words, of which the root seems to be the gutturalcca, coming from far back in the throat;ccallani, ‘to break,’ccatatani, ‘to gnash the teeth,’ccacñiy, ‘thunder,’ and the expressive words for ‘a thunder-storm,’ccaccaccahay, which carries the imitative process so much farther than such European words as thunder-clap, donner-klapf. In Maori,patais ‘topatteras water dropping, drops of rain.’ The Manchu language describes the noise of fruits falling from the trees aspata pata(so Hindustanibhadbhad); this is like our wordpat, and we should say in the same manner that the fruit comespatteringdown, while Frenchpatatrais a recognized imitation of something falling. Copticpotptis ‘to fall,’ and the Australianbadbadin(orpatpatin) is translated into almost literal English aspitpatting. On the strength of such non-Aryan languages, are we to assign an imitative origin to the Sanskrit verb-rootpat, ‘to fall,’ and to Greekπίπτω?
Wishing rather to gain a clear survey of the principles of language-making than to plunge into obscure problems, it is not necessary for me to discuss here questions of intricate detail. The point which continually arises is this,—granted that a particular kind of transition from sound to sense is possible in the abstract, may it be safely claimed in a particular case? In looking through the vocabularies of the world, it appears that most languages offer words which, by obvious likeliness or by their correspondence with similar forms elsewhere, may put forward a tolerable claim to be considered imitative. Some languages, as Aztec or Mohawk, offer singularly few examples, while in others they are much more numerous. Take Australian cases:walle, ‘towail;’bung-bung-ween, ‘thunder;’wirriti, ‘to blow, as wind;’wirrirriti, ‘to storm, rage, as in fight;’wirri,bwirri, ‘the native throwing club,’ seemingly so called from itswhirthrough the air;kurarriti, ‘to hum, buzz;’kurrirrurriri, ‘round about, unintelligible,’ &c.;pitata, ‘to knock, pelt, as rain,’pitapitata, ‘to knock;’wiiti, ‘to laugh, rejoice’—as in our own ‘Turnament of Tottenham’:—
‘“We te he!” quoth Tyb, and lugh,“Ye er a dughty man!”’
‘“We te he!” quoth Tyb, and lugh,“Ye er a dughty man!”’
‘“We te he!” quoth Tyb, and lugh,“Ye er a dughty man!”’
‘“We te he!” quoth Tyb, and lugh,
“Ye er a dughty man!”’
The so-called Chinook Jargon of British Columbia is a language crowded with imitative words, sometimes adopted from the native Indian languages, sometimes made on the spot by the combined efforts of the white man and the Indian to make one another understand. Samples of its quality arehóh-hoh, ‘to cough,’kó-ko, ‘to knock,’kwa-lal-kwa-lal, ‘to gallop,’muck-a-muck, ‘to eat,’chak-chak, ‘the bald eagle’ (from its scream),mamook tsish(maketsish), ‘to sharpen on the grindstone.’ It has been remarked byProf.Max Müller that the peculiar sound made in blowing out a candle is not a favourite in civilized languages, but it seems to be recognized here, for no doubt it is what the compiler of the vocabulary is doing his best to write down when he givesmamook poh(makepoh) as the Chinook expression for ‘to blow out or extinguish as a candle.’ This jargon is in great measure of new growth within the last seventy or eighty years, but its imitative words do not differ in nature from those of the more ordinary and old-established languages of the world. Thus among Brazilian tribes there appear Tupicororóng,cururuc, ‘to snore’ (compare Coptickherkher, Quichuaccorcuni(ccor)), whence it appears that an imitation of a snore may perhaps serve the Carajás Indians to express ‘to sleep’ asarourou-cré, as well as the related idea of ‘night,’roou. Again Pimenteiraebaung, ‘to bruise, beat,’ compares with Yorubagba, ‘to slap,’gbã(gbang) ‘to sound loudly, tobang,’ and so forth. Among African languages, the Zulu seems particularly rich in imitative words. Thusbibiza, ‘to dribble like children, drivel in speaking’ (compare Englishbib);babala, ‘the larger bush-antelope’ (from thebaaof the female);boba, ‘tobabble, chatter, be noisy,’bobi, ‘ababbler;’boboni, ‘a throstle’ (criesbo! bo!compareAmericanbobolink);bomboloza, ‘to rumble in the bowels, to have a bowel-complaint;’bubula, ‘tobuzzlike bees,’bubulela, ‘a swarm of bees, a buzzing crowd of people;’bubuluza, ‘to make a blustering noise, like frothing beer or boiling fat.’ These examples, from among those given under one initial letter in one dictionary of one barbaric language, may give an idea of the amount of the evidence from the languages of the lower races bearing on the present problem.
For the present purpose of giving a brief series of examples of the sort of words in which imitative sound seems fairly traceable, the strongest and most manageable evidence is of course found among such words as directly describe sounds or what produces them, such as cries of and names for animals, the terms for action accompanied by sound, and the materials and objects so acted upon. In further investigation it becomes more and more requisite to isolate the sound-type or root from the modifications and additions to which it has been subjected for grammatical and phonetical adaptation. It will serve to give an idea of the extent and intricacy of this problem, to glance at a group of words in one European language, and notice the etymological network which spreads round the German wordklapf, in Grimm’s dictionary,klappen,klippen,klopfen,kläffen,klimpern,klampern,klateren,kloteren,klitteren,klatzen,klacken, and more, to be matched with allied forms in other languages. Setting aside the consideration of grammatical inflexion, it belongs to the present subject to notice that man’s imitative faculty in language is by no means limited to making direct copies of sound and shaping them into words. It seizes upon ready-made terms of whatever origin, alters and adapts them to make their sound fitting to their sense, and pours into the dictionaries a flood of adapted words of which the most difficult to analyse are those which are neither altogether etymological nor altogether imitative, but partly both. How words, while preserving,so to speak, the same skeleton, may be made to follow the variation of sound, of force, of duration, of size, an imitative group more or less connected with the last will show—crick,creak,crack,crash,crush,crunch,craunch,scrunch,scraunch. It does not at all follow that because a word suffers such imitative and symbolic changes it must be, like this, directly imitative in its origin. What, for instance, could sound more imitative than the name of that old-fashioned cannon for throwing grape-shot, thepatterero? Yet the etymology of the word appears in the Spanish formpedrero, Frenchperrier; it means simply an instrument for throwing stones (piedra,pierre), and it was only when the Spanish word was adopted in England that the imitative faculty caught and transformed it into an apparent sound-word, resembling the verb topatter. The propensity of language, especially in slang, to make sense of strange words by altering them into something with an appropriate meaning has been often dwelt upon by philologists, but the propensity to alter words into something with an appropriate sound has produced results immensely more important. The effects of symbolic change of sound acting upon verb-roots seem almost boundless. The verb towaddlehas a strong imitative appearance, and so in German we can hardly resist the suggestion that imitative sound has to do with the difference betweenwandernandwandeln; but all these verbs belong to a family represented by Sanskritvad, to go, Latinvado, and to this root there seems no sufficient ground for assigning an imitative origin, the traces of which it has at any rate lost if it ever had them. Thus, again, tostampwith the foot, which has been claimed as an imitation of sound, seems only a ‘coloured’ word. The rootsta, ‘to stand,’ Sanskritsthâ, forms a causativestap, Sanskritsthâpay, ‘to make to stand,’ English tostop, and a foot-stepis when the foot comes to a stand, a foot-stop. But we have Anglo-Saxonstapan,stæpan,steppan, English tostep, varying to express its meaning by sound in tostaup, tostamp, tostump, andtostomp, contrasting in their violence or clumsy weight with the foot on the Dorset cottage-sill in Barnes’s poem:—
‘Where love do seek the maïden’s evenèn vloor,Wi’stip-step light, an tip-tap slightAgeän the door.’
‘Where love do seek the maïden’s evenèn vloor,Wi’stip-step light, an tip-tap slightAgeän the door.’
‘Where love do seek the maïden’s evenèn vloor,Wi’stip-step light, an tip-tap slightAgeän the door.’
‘Where love do seek the maïden’s evenèn vloor,
Wi’stip-step light, an tip-tap slight
Ageän the door.’
By expanding, modifying, or, so to speak, colouring, sound is able to produce effects closely like those of gesture-language, expressing length or shortness of time, strength or weakness of action, and then passing into a further stage to describe greatness or smallness of size or of distance, and thence making its way into the widest fields of metaphor. And it does all this with a force which is surprising when we consider how childishly simple are the means employed. Thus the Bachapin of Africa call a man with the cryhéla!but according as he is far or farther off the sound of thehêela!hê-ê-la!is lengthened out. Mr. Macgregor in his ‘Rob Roy on the Jordan,’ graphically describes this method of expression, ‘“But where is Zalmouda?”... Then with rough eagerness the strongest of the Dowana faction pushes his long forefinger forward, pointing straight enough—but whither? and with a volley of words ends,Ah-ah-a-a-a——a-a. This strange expression had long before puzzled me when first heard from a shepherd in Bashan.... But the simple meaning of this long string of “ah’s” shortened, and quickened, and lowered in tone to the end, is merely that the place pointed to is a “very great way off.”’ The Chinook Jargon, as usual representing primitive developments of language, uses a similar device in lengthening the sound of words to indicate distance. The Siamese can, by varying the tone-accent, make the syllablenon, ‘there,’ express a near, indefinite, or far distance, and in like manner can modify the meaning of such a word asny, ‘little.’ In the Gaboon, the strength with which such a word asmpolu, ‘great,’ is uttered serves to show whether it is great, very great, or very very great, and in this way, as Mr. Wilson remarks in his Mpongwe Grammar, ‘thecomparative degrees of greatness, smallness, hardness, rapidity, and strength, &c., may be conveyed with more accuracy and precision than could readily be conceived.’ In Madagascarratchimeans ‘bad,’ butrâtchiis ‘very bad.’ The natives of Australia, according to Oldfield, show the use of this process in combination with that of symbolic reduplication: among the Watchandie tribejir-riesignifies ‘already or past,’jir-rie jir-rieindicates ‘a long time ago,’ whilejie-r-rie jirrie(the first syllable being dwelt on for some time) signifies ‘an immense time ago.’ Again,boo-rieis ‘small,’boo-rie-boo-rie‘very small,’ andb-o-rie boorie‘exceedingly small.’ Wilhelm von Humboldt notices the habit of the southern Guarani dialect of South America of dwelling more or less time on the suffix of the perfect tense,yma,y—ma, to indicate the length or shortness of the distance of time at which the action took place; and it is curious to observe that a similar contrivance is made use of among the aboriginal tribes of India, where the Ho language forms a future tense by addingáto the root, and prolonging its sound,kajee‘to speak,’ Amgkajēēá‘I will speak.’ As might be expected, the languages of very rude tribes show extremely well how the results of such primitive processes pass into the recognized stock of language. Nothing could be better for this than the words by which one of the rudest of living races, the Botocudos of Brazil, express the sea. They have a word for a stream,ouatou, and an adjective which means great,ijipakijiou; thence the two words ‘stream-great,’ a little strengthened in the vowels, will give the term for a river,ouatou-ijiipakiiijou, as it were, ‘stream-grea-at,’ and this, to express the immensity of the ocean, is amplified intoouatou-iijipakiijou-ou-ou-ou-ou-ou. Another tribe of the same family works out the same result more simply; the wordouatou, ‘stream,’ becomesouatou-ou-ou-ou, ‘the sea.’ The Chavantes very naturally stretch the expressionrom-o-wodi, ‘I go a long way,’ intorom-o-o-o-o-wodi, ‘I go a very long way indeed,’ and when they are called upon to countbeyond five they say it iska-o-o-oki, by which they evidently mean it is a very great many. The Cauixanas in one vocabulary are described as sayinglawauugabifor four, and drawling out the same word for five, as if to say ‘a long four,’ in somewhat the same way as the Aponegicrans, whose word for six isitawuna, can expand this into a word for seven,itawuūna, obviously thus meaning a ‘long six.’ In their earlier and simpler stages nothing can be more easy to comprehend than these, so to speak, pictorial modifications of words. It is true that writing, even with the aid of italics and capitals, ignores much of this symbolism in spoken language, but every child can see its use and meaning, in spite of the efforts of book-learning and school-teaching to set aside whatever cannot be expressed by their imperfect symbols, nor controlled by their narrow rules. But when we try to follow out to their full results these methods, at first so easy to trace and appreciate, we soon find them passing out of our grasp. The language of the Sahaptin Indians shows us a process of modifying words which is far from clear, and yet not utterly obscure. These Indians have a way of making a kind of disrespectful diminutive by changing thenin a word tol; thustwinwtmeans ‘tailless,’ but to indicate particular smallness, or to express contempt, they make this intotwilwt, pronounced with an appropriate change of tone; and again,wanameans ‘river,’ but this is made into a diminutivewalaby ‘changingnintol, giving the voice a different tone, putting the lips out in speaking, and keeping them suspended around the jaw.’ Here we are told enough about the change of pronunciation to guess at least how it could convey the notions of smallness and contempt. But it is less easy to follow the process by which the Mpongwe language turns an affirmative into a negative verb by ‘an intonation upon, or prolongation of the radical vowel,’tŏnda, to love,tŏnda, not to love;tŏndo, to be loved,tŏndo, not to be loved. So Yoruba,bába, ‘a great thing,’bàba, ‘a small thing,’ contrasted in a proverb, ‘Bababo,babamolle’—‘A greatmatter puts a smaller out of sight.’ Language is, in fact, full of phonetic modifications which justify a suspicion that symbolic sound had to do with their production, though it may be hard to say exactly how.
Again, there is the familiar process of reduplication, simple or modified, which produces such forms asmurmur,pitpat,helterskelter. This action, though much restricted in literary dialects, has such immense scope in the talk of children and savages that Professor Pott’s treatise on it[290]has become incidentally one of the most valuable collections of facts ever made with relation to early stages of language. Now up to a certain point any child can see how and why such doubling is done, and how it always adds something to the original idea. It may make superlatives or otherwise intensify words, as in Polynesialoa‘long,’lololoa‘very long’; Mandingoding‘a child,’dingding‘a very little child.’ It makes plurals, as Malayraja-raja‘princes,’orang-orang‘people.’ It adds numerals, as Mosquitowalwal‘four’ (two-two), or distributes them, as Copticouai ouai‘singly’ (one-one). These are cases where the motive of doubling is comparatively easy to make out. As an example of cases much more difficult to comprehend may be taken the familiar reduplication of the perfect tense, Greekγέγραφαfromγράφω, Latinmomordifrommordeo, Gothichaihaldfromhaldan, ‘to hold.’ Reduplication is habitually used in imitative words to intensify them, and still more, to show that the sound is repeated or continuous. From the immense mass of such words we may take as instances the Botocudohou-hou-hou-gitcha‘to suck’ (compare Tonganhūhū‘breast’),kiaku-käck-käck, ‘a butterfly’; Quichuachiuiuiuiñichi‘wind whistling in the trees’; Maoriharuru‘noise of wind’;hohoro‘hurry’; Dayakkakakkaka‘to go on laughing loud’; Ainoshiriushiriukanni‘a rasp’; Tamilmurumuru‘tomurmur’; Akraewiewiewiewie‘he spoke repeatedlyand continually’; and so on, throughout the whole range of the languages of the world.
The device of conveying different ideas of distance by the use of a graduated scale of vowels seems to me one of great philological interest, from the suggestive hint it gives of the proceedings of the language-makers in most distant regions of the world, working out in various ways a similar ingenious contrivance of expression by sound. A typical series is the Javan:iki‘this’ (close by)ika‘that’ (at some distance);iku‘that’ (farther off). It is not likely that the following list nearly exhausts the whole number of cases in the languages of the world, for about half the number have been incidentally noted down by myself without any especial search, but merely in the course of looking over vocabularies of the lower races.[291]
Javan ...iki, this;ika, that (intermediate);iku, that.
Malagasy ...ao, there (at a short distance);eo, there (at a shorter distance);io, there (close at hand).atsy, there (not far off);etsy, there (nearer);itsy, this or these.
Japanese ...ko, here;ka, there.korera, these;karera, they (those).
Canarese ...ivanu, this;uvanu, that (intermediate);avanu, that.
Tamul ...î, this;â, that.
Rajmahali ...îh, this;âh, that.
Dhimal ...isho,ita, here;usho,uta, there.iti,idong, this;uti,udong, that (of things and persons respectively).
Abchasian ...abri, this;ubri, that.
Ossetic ...am, here;um, there.
Magyar ...ez, this;az, that.
Zulu ...apa, here;apo, there.lesi,leso,lesiya;abu,abo,abuya; &c. = this, that, that (in the distance).
Yoruba ...na, this;ni, that.
Fernandian ...olo, this;ole, that.
Tumale ...re, this;ri, that.ngi, I;ngo, thou;ngu, he.
Greenlandish ...uv, here, there (where one points to);iv, there, up there.
Sujelpa (Coleville Ind.) ...aa, this;ii, that.
Sahaptin ...kina, here;kuna, there.
Mutsun ...ne, here;nu, there.
Tarahumara ...ibe, here;abe, there.
Guarani ...nde,ne, thou;ndi,ni, he.
Botocudo ...ati, I;oti, thou, you, (prep.) to.
Carib ...ne, thou;ni, he.
Chilian ...tva,vachi, this;tvey,veychi, that.
It is obvious on inspection of this list of pronouns and adverbs that they have in some way come to have their vowels contrasted to match the contrast of here and there, this and that. Accident may sometimes account for such cases. For instance it is well known to philologists that our ownthisandthatare pronouns partly distinct in their formation,thi-sbeing probably two pronouns run together, but yet the Dutch neutersdit‘this,’ anddat‘that,’ have taken the appearance of a single form with contrasted vowels.[292]But accident cannot account for the frequency of such words in pairs, and even in sets of three, in so many different languages. There must have been some common intention at work, and there is evidence that some of these languages do resort to a change of sound as a means of expressing change of distance. Thus the language of Fernando Po can not only express ‘this’ and ‘that’ byolo,ole, but it can even make a change of the pronunciation of the vowel distinguish betweeno boehe‘this month,’ andoh boehe, ‘that month.’ In the same way the Grebo can make the difference between ‘I’ and ‘thou,’ ‘we,’ and ‘you,’ ‘solely by the intonation of the voice, which the finalhof the second personsmâhandăhis intended to express.’