LECTURE X
THE ORIGIN OF FLOWERS
Introduction—Precursors of Darwin—Pollination by wind—Arrangements in flowers for securing cross-fertilization—Salvia, Pedicularis—Flowers visited by flies—Aristolochia—Pinguicula—Daphne—Orchids—Flowers are built up of adaptations—Mouth-parts of insects—Proboscis of butterflies—Mouth-parts of the cockroach—Of the bee—Pollen baskets of bees—Origin of flowers—Attraction of insects by colour—Limitation of the area visited—Nägeli's objection to the theory of selection—Other interpretations excluded—Viola calcarata—Only those changes which are useful to their possessors have persisted—Deceptive flowers—Cypripedium—Pollinia of Orchis—The case of the Yucca-moth—The relative imperfection of the adaptations tells in favour of their origin through natural selection—Honey thieves.
Introduction—Precursors of Darwin—Pollination by wind—Arrangements in flowers for securing cross-fertilization—Salvia, Pedicularis—Flowers visited by flies—Aristolochia—Pinguicula—Daphne—Orchids—Flowers are built up of adaptations—Mouth-parts of insects—Proboscis of butterflies—Mouth-parts of the cockroach—Of the bee—Pollen baskets of bees—Origin of flowers—Attraction of insects by colour—Limitation of the area visited—Nägeli's objection to the theory of selection—Other interpretations excluded—Viola calcarata—Only those changes which are useful to their possessors have persisted—Deceptive flowers—Cypripedium—Pollinia of Orchis—The case of the Yucca-moth—The relative imperfection of the adaptations tells in favour of their origin through natural selection—Honey thieves.
Whenone species is so intimately bound up with another that neither can live for any length of time except in partnership, that is certainly an example of far-reaching mutual adaptation, but there are innumerable cases of mutual adaptation, in which, although there is no common life in the same place, yet the first form of life is adjusted in relation to the peculiarities of the second, and the second to those of the first. One of the most beautiful, and, in regard to natural selection, the most instructive of these cases is illustrated by the relations between insects and the higher plants, relations which have grown out of the fact that many insects have formed the habit of visiting the flowers of the plants for the sake of the pollen. In this connexion the theory of selection has made the most unexpected and highly interesting disclosures, for it has informed us how the flowers have arisen.
In earlier times the beauty, the splendour of colour, and the fragrance of flowers were regarded as phenomena created for the delight of mankind, or as an outcome of the infinite creative power of Mother Nature, who loves to run riot in form and colour. Without allowing our pleasure in all this manifold beauty to be spoilt, we must nowadays form quite a different conception of the way in which the flowers have been called into being. Although here, as everywhere else in Nature, we cannot go back to ultimate causes, yet we can show, on very satisfactory evidence, that the flowers illustrate the reaction of the plants to the visits of insects, and that they have been in large measure evoked by these visits. There might, indeed,have been blossoms, but there would have been no flowers—that is to say, blossoms with large, coloured, outer parts, with fragrance, and with nectar inside, unless the blossoms had been sought out by insects during the long ages. Flowers are adaptations of the higher flowering plants to the visits of insects. There can be no doubt about that now, for—thanks to the numerous and very detailed studies of a small number of prominent workers—we need not only suppose it, we can prove it with all the certainty that can be desired. The mutual adaptations of insects and flowers afford one of the clearest examples of the mode of operation and the power of natural selection, and the case cannot therefore be omitted from lectures on the theory of descent.
That bees and many other insects visit flowers for the sake of the nectar and pollen has been known to men from very early times. But this fact by itself would only explain why adaptations to flower-visiting have taken place in these insects to enable them, for instance, to reach the nectar out of deep corolla-tubes, or to load themselves with a great quantity of pollen, and to carry it to their hives, as happens in the case of the bees. But what causes the plants to produce nectar, and offer it to the insects, since it is of no use to themselves? And further, what induces them to make the pillage easier to the insects, by making their blossoms visible from afar through their brilliant colours, or by sending forth a stream of fragrance that, even during the night, guides their visitors towards them?
As far back as the end of the eighteenth century a thoughtful and clear-sighted Berlin naturalist, Christian Konrad Sprengel, took a great step towards answering this question. In the year 1793 he published a paper entitled 'The Newly Discovered Secret of Nature in the Structure and Fertilization of Flowers[8],' in which he quite correctly recognized and interpreted a great many of the remarkable adaptations of flowers to the visits of insects. Unfortunately, the value of these discoveries was not appreciated in Sprengel's own time, and his work had to wait more than half a century for recognition.
[8]Das neu-entdeckte Geheimniss der Natur im Bau u. der Befruchtung der Blumen, Berlin, 1793.
[8]Das neu-entdeckte Geheimniss der Natur im Bau u. der Befruchtung der Blumen, Berlin, 1793.
Sprengel was completely dominated by the idea of an all-wise Creator, who 'has not created even a single hair without intention,' and, guided by this idea, he endeavoured to penetrate into the significance of many little details in the structure of flowers. Thus he recognized that the hairs which cover the lower surface of thepetals of the wood-cranesbill (Geranium sylvaticum) protect the nectar of the flower from being diluted with rain, and he drew the conclusion, correct enough, though far removed from our modern ideas as regards the directly efficient cause, that the nectar was there for the insects.
He was also impressed by the fact that the sky-blue corolla of the forget-me-not (Myosotis palustris) has a beautiful yellow ring round the entrance to the corolla-tube, and he interpreted this as a means by which insects were shown the way to the nectar which is concealed in the depths of the tube.
Fig. 40.Potentilla verna, after Hermann Müller.A,seen from above.Kbl, sepals.Bl, petals.Nt, nectariesnear the base of the stamens.B, section through theflower.Gr, stigma.St, stamen.Nt, nectary.
Fig. 40.Potentilla verna, after Hermann Müller.A,seen from above.Kbl, sepals.Bl, petals.Nt, nectariesnear the base of the stamens.B, section through theflower.Gr, stigma.St, stamen.Nt, nectary.
We now know that such 'honey-guides' are present in most of the flowers visited by insects, in the form of spots, lines, or other marking, usually of conspicuous colour, that is, of a colour contrasting with the ground colour of the flower. Thus, in species of Iris, regular paths of short hairs lead the way to the place where the nectar lies. In the spring potentilla (Potentilla verna) (Fig. 40) the yellow petals (A,Bl) become bright orange-red towards their bases, and this shows the way to the nectaries, which lie at the bases of the stamens (st), and are protected by hairs, the so-called 'nectar-covers' (Saftdecke) of Sprengel, from being washed by rain.
The recognition of the honey-guides led Sprengel on to the idea that the general colouring of the flower effects on a large scale what the honey-guides do in a more detailed way—it attracts the attention of passing insects to where nectar is to be found; indeed, he went an important step further by recognizing that there are flowers which cannot fertilize themselves, in which the insect, in its search for honey, covers itself with pollen, which is then rubbed off on the stigma of the next flower visited, fertilization being thus effected. He demonstrated this not only for the Iris, but for many other flowers, and he drew the conclusion that 'Nature does not seem to have wished that any flower should be fertilized by its own pollen.'How near Sprengel was to reaching a complete solution of the problem is now plain to us, for he even discovered that many flowers, such asHemerocallis fulva, remained infertile if they were dusted with their own pollen.
Even the numerous experiments of that admirable German botanist, C. F. Gärtner, although they advanced matters further, did not suffice to make the relations between insects and flowers thoroughly clear; for this the basis of the theory of Descent and Selection was necessary. Here, again, it was reserved for Charles Darwin to lead the way where both contemporaries and predecessors had been blindly groping. He recognized that,in general, self-fertilization is disadvantageous to plants; that they produce fewer seeds, and that these produce feebler plants, than when they are cross-fertilized; that, therefore, those flowers which are arranged to secure cross-fertilization have an advantage over those which are self-fertilized. In many species, as Sprengel had already pointed out, self-fertilization leads to actual infertility; only a few plants are as fertile with their own pollen as with that of another plant; and Darwin believed that, in all flowering plants, crossing with others of the same kind, at least from time to time, is necessary if they are not to degenerate.
Thus the advantage which the flowers derive from the visits of insects lies in the fact that insects are instrumental in the cross-fertilization of the flowers, and we can now understand how the plant was able to vary in a manner favourable to the insect-visits, and to exhibit adaptations which serve exclusively to make these visits easier; we understand how it was possible that there should develop among flowers an endless number of contrivances which served solely to attract insects, and even how, for the same end, the insignificant blossoms of the oldest Phanerogams must have been transformed into real flowers.
We must not imagine, however, that the obviously important crossing of plant-individuals, usually called 'cross-pollination,' can be effected only by means of insects. There were numerous plants in earlier times, and there is still a whole series in which cross-fertilization is effected through the air by the wind; these are the anemophilous or wind-pollinated Angiosperms.
To these belong most of the catkin-bearers, such as hazel and birch, and also the grasses and sedges, the hemp and the hop, and so forth. In these plants there is no real flower, but only an inconspicuous blossom, without brightly-coloured outer envelopes, without fragrance or nectar; all of them have smooth pollen grains, whicheasily separate into fine dust and are carried away by the wind until they fall, by chance, far from their place of origin, on the stigma of a female blossom.
Fig. 41.Flower of Meadow Sage (Salvia pratensis),after H. Müller.st´, immature anthers concealedin the 'helmet' of the flower.st´´, mature antherlowered.gr´, immature stigma.gr´´, maturestigma.U, the lower lip of the corolla, thelanding-stage for the bee.
Fig. 41.Flower of Meadow Sage (Salvia pratensis),after H. Müller.st´, immature anthers concealedin the 'helmet' of the flower.st´´, mature antherlowered.gr´, immature stigma.gr´´, maturestigma.U, the lower lip of the corolla, thelanding-stage for the bee.
By far the greater number of the phanerogams, however, especially all our indigenous 'flowers,' are, as a rule, fertilized by means of insects, and it is amazing to see in what diverse ways, often highly specialized, they have adapted themselves to the visits of insects. Thus there are flowers in which the nectar lies open to view, and these can be feasted on by all manner of insects; there are others in which the nectar is rather more concealed, but still easily found, and reached by insects with short mouth-parts, e.g. large flowers blooming by day and bearing much pollen, like the Magnolias. These have been called beetle-flowers, because they are visited especially by the honey-loving Longicorns.
Other flowers blooming by day are especially adapted to fertilization by means of bees; they are always beautifully coloured, often blue; they are fragrant, and contain nectar deep down in the flower, where it can only be reached by the comparatively long proboscis of the bee. Different arrangements in the different flowers secure that the bee cannot enjoy the nectar without at the same time effecting the cross-pollination. Thus the stamens of the meadow sage (Salvia pratensis) are at first hidden within the helmet-shaped upper lip of the flower (Fig. 41,st´), but bear lower down on their stalk a short handle-like process, which turns the pollen-bearing anther downwards (st´´) as soon as it is pressed back by an intruding insect. The pollen-sacs then strike downwards on the back of the bee, and cover it with pollen. When the bee visits another more mature flower, the long style, which was at first hidden within the helmet, has bent downwards (gr´´), and now stands just in front of the entrance to the flower, so that the bee must rub off a part of the pollen covering its back on to the stigma, and fertilization is thus effected.
There are other flowers which are specially disposed to suit thevisits of the humble-bees, as, for instance,Pedicularis asplenifolia, the fern-leaved louse-wort, a plant of the high Alps (Fig. 42). The first thing that strikes us about this plant is the thickly tufted hair covering on the calyx (k), which serves to keep off little wingless insects from the flower; then there is the strange left-sided twisting of the individual flowers, whose under lip allows only a strong insect like the humble-bee to gain access, towards the left, to the corolla-tube (kr), in the depths of which the nectar is concealed. While the humble-bee is sucking up the nectar it becomes dusted over with pollen from the anthers, which falls to dust at a touch, and when it insinuates itself into a second flower its powdered back comes first into contact with the stigma of the pistil (gr) which projects from the elongated bill-shaped under lip, dusting it over with the pollen of the first visited flower. Butterflies and smaller bees cannot rob this flower; it is strictly a humble-bee's flower.
Fig. 42.Alpine Lousewort (Pedicularis asplenifolia).A, flower seen from the left side, enlarged three times; the arrows show the path by which the humble-bee enters.B, the same flower, seen from the left, after removal of the calyx, the lower lip and the left half of the upper lip.C, ovary (ov), nectary (n), and base of style.D, tip of style, bearing the stigma.E, two anthers turned towards one another.o, upper lip.u, lower lip.gr, style.st, anthers.kr, corolla-tube.k, calyx.
Fig. 42.Alpine Lousewort (Pedicularis asplenifolia).A, flower seen from the left side, enlarged three times; the arrows show the path by which the humble-bee enters.B, the same flower, seen from the left, after removal of the calyx, the lower lip and the left half of the upper lip.C, ovary (ov), nectary (n), and base of style.D, tip of style, bearing the stigma.E, two anthers turned towards one another.o, upper lip.u, lower lip.gr, style.st, anthers.kr, corolla-tube.k, calyx.
There are not a few of such flowers adapted to a very restricted circle of visitors, and in all of them we find contrivances which close the entrance to all except what we may call the welcome insects; sometimes there are cushions of bristles which prevent little insects from creeping up from below, or it is the oblique position of the flower which prevents their getting in from the stem; sometimes it is the length and narrowness of the corolla-tube, or the deep and hidden situation of the nectar, which only allows intelligent insects to find the treasure.
Very remarkable are those flowers which are adapted to the visits of flies, for they correspond in several respects to the peculiarities of these insects. In the first place, flies are fond of decaying substances and the odours given off by these, and so the flowers which depend for their cross-fertilization on flies have taken on the dull and ugly colours of decay, and give out a disagreeable smell. But flies are also shy and restless, turning now hither, now thither, and cannot be reckoned among the 'constant' insect visitors, that is to say, they do not persistently visit the same species; it is, therefore, evident that they might easily carry away the pollen without any useful result ensuing. Moreover, their intelligence is of a low order, and they do not seek nectar with the perseverance shown by bees and humble-bees. It is not surprising, therefore, to find that many of the flowers adapted for the visits of flies are so constructed that they detain their visitors until they have done their duty, that is to say, until they have effected, or at least begun, the process of cross-pollination.
Fig. 43.Flower of Birthwort (Aristolochiaclematitis) cut in half.A, beforepollination by small flies.b, thebristles.B, after pollination.P, pollenmass.N, stigma,b, the bristles.b´, their remains. After H. Müller.
Fig. 43.Flower of Birthwort (Aristolochiaclematitis) cut in half.A, beforepollination by small flies.b, thebristles.B, after pollination.P, pollenmass.N, stigma,b, the bristles.b´, their remains. After H. Müller.
Fig. 44.Alpine Butterwort(Pinguicula alpina).A, section through the flower.K, calyx.bh, bristly prominences.sp, spur.st, stamen.n, stigma.B, stigma andstamen more magnified.After H. Müller.
Fig. 44.Alpine Butterwort(Pinguicula alpina).A, section through the flower.K, calyx.bh, bristly prominences.sp, spur.st, stamen.n, stigma.B, stigma andstamen more magnified.After H. Müller.
Our birthwort (Aristolochia clematitis) and the Cuckoo-pint (Arum maculatum) are pit-fall flowers, whose long corolla-tubes havean enlargement at the base, in which both pistil and stamens are contained. In the birthwort (Fig. 43) the narrow entrance-tube is thickly beset with stiff hairs (A,b), whose points are all directed towards the base. Little flies can creep down quite comfortably into the basal expansion, but once there they are kept imprisoned until the flower, in consequence of the pollination of the stigma, begins to wither, the first parts to go being these very bristles (B,b´), whose points, like a fish-weir, prevented the flies from creeping out. Other 'fly-flowers,' as for instance the Alpine butterwort (Pinguicula alpina) (Fig. 44), securely imprison the plump fly as soon as it has succeeded in forcing itself in far enough to reach, with its short proboscis, the nectar contained in the spur (sp) of the corolla. The backward-directed bristles hold it fast for some time, and it is only by hard pressing with the back against the anthers (st) lying above it, and against the stigma (n), that it ultimately succeeds in getting free, but it never does so without having either loaded itself with pollen, or rubbed off on the stigma the pollen it brought with it from another similar flower. The Alpine butterwort is protogynous, that is to say, the pistil ripens first, the pollen later, so that the possibility of self-fertilization is altogether excluded.
It would be impossible to give even an approximate idea of the diversity of the contrivances for securing fertilization in flowers without spending many hours over them, for they are different in almost every flower, often widely so, and even in species of the same genus they are by no means always alike; for not infrequently one species is adapted to one circle of visitors, and its near relative to another. Thus the flower of the common Daphne (Daphne mezereum) (Fig. 45,AandC) is adapted to the visits of butterflies, bees, and hover-flies, while its nearest relative (Daphne striata) (Fig. 45,BandD) has a somewhat narrower and longer corolla-tube, so that only butterflies can feast upon it. This example shows that there are exclusively 'butterfly flowers,' but specialization goes further, for there are flowers adapted to diurnal and others to nocturnal Lepidoptera. The former have usually bright, often red colours, and a pleasant aromatic fragrance, and in all of them the nectar lies at the bottom of a very narrow corolla-tube. To this class belong, for instance, the species of pink, many orchids, such asOrchis ustulata, andNigritella angustifoliaof the Alps, which smells strongly of vanilla; also the beautiful campion (Lychnis diurna) and the Alpine primrose (Primula farinosa). The flowers adapted to nocturnal Lepidoptera are characterized by pale, often white colour, and a strong and pleasant smell, which only begins to stream out after sunset, and indeed many of these flowersare quite closed by day. This is the case with the large, white, scentless bindweed (Convolvulus sepium), which is chiefly visited and fertilized by the largest of our hawk-moths (Sphinx convolvuli). The pale soapwort (Saponaria officinalis) exhales a delicate fragrance which attracts the Sphingidæ from afar, and the sweet smell of the honeysuckle (Lonicera periclymenum) is well known, and has the same effect; an arbour of honeysuckle often attracts whole companies of our most beautiful Sphingidæ and Noctuidæ on warm June nights, to the great delight of the moth-collecting youth.
Fig. 45.Daphne mezereum(AandC) and Daphne striata (BandD). The former visited by butterflies, bees, and flies, the latter by butterflies only.AandB, vertical sections of the flowers.St, stamens.Gr, style.n, nectary.CandD, flowers seen from above. After H. Müller.
Fig. 45.Daphne mezereum(AandC) and Daphne striata (BandD). The former visited by butterflies, bees, and flies, the latter by butterflies only.AandB, vertical sections of the flowers.St, stamens.Gr, style.n, nectary.CandD, flowers seen from above. After H. Müller.
I cannot conclude this account of flower-adaptations without considering the orchids somewhat more in detail, for it is among them that we find the most far-reaching adaptations to the visits of insects. Among them, too, great diversity prevails, as is evident from the fact that Darwin devoted a whole book to the arrangements for fertilization in orchids, but the main features are very much the same in the majority. Figure 46 gives a representation of one of our commonest species (Orchis mascula), A shows the flower in side view,Bas it appears from in front. The flower seems as it were to float on the end of the stalk (st), stretching out horizontally the spur (sp) which contains the nectar. Between the large, broad under lip (U), marked with a honey-guide (sm), and offering a convenient alighting surface, and the broad, cushion-like stigma (n) lies the entrance to the spur. Fertilization occurs in the following way:—The fly or bee, when it is in the act of pushing its proboscis into the nectar-containing spur,knocks with its head against the so-called rostellum (r), a little beak-like process at the base of the stamens (p). The pollen masses are of very peculiar construction, not falling to dust, but forming little stalked clubs, with the pollen grains glued together, and so arranged that they spring off when the rostellum is touched and attach themselves to the head of the insect, as atDon the pencil (Fig. 46). When the bee has sucked up the nectar out of the spur, and then proceeds to penetrate into another flower of the same species, the pollinia have bent downwards on its forehead (E), and must unfailingly come in contact with the stigma of the second flower, to which they now remain attached, and effect its fertilization. What a long chain of purposeful arrangements in a single flower, and no interpretation of them is available except through natural selection!
Fig. 46.Common Orchis (Orchis mascula).A, flower in side view.st, stalk.sp, spur with the nectary (n).ei, entrance to the spur.U, lower lip.B, flower from in front.p, pollinia.Sm, honey-guide.ei, entrance to the nectar.na, stigma.r, rostellum.U, lower lip.C, vertical section through the rostellum (r), pollinium (p).ei, entrance.D, the pollinia removed and standing erect on the tip of a lead-pencil.E, the same, somewhat later, curved downwards.
Fig. 46.Common Orchis (Orchis mascula).A, flower in side view.st, stalk.sp, spur with the nectary (n).ei, entrance to the spur.U, lower lip.B, flower from in front.p, pollinia.Sm, honey-guide.ei, entrance to the nectar.na, stigma.r, rostellum.U, lower lip.C, vertical section through the rostellum (r), pollinium (p).ei, entrance.D, the pollinia removed and standing erect on the tip of a lead-pencil.E, the same, somewhat later, curved downwards.
And how diversely are these again modified in the different genera and species of orchids, of which one is adapted to the visits of butterflies exclusively, asOrchis ustulata, another to those of bees, asOrchis morio, and a third to those of flies, asOphrys muscifera. These flowers are adapted to insect visits in the minutest details of the form of the petals, which are smooth, as if polished with wax, where insects are not intended to creep, but velvety or hairy where the path leadsto the nectar, and at the same time to the pollen and the stigma. And then there is the diversity in the form and colour of the 'honey-guides' on the 'alighting surface,' that is, the under lip of the flower, upon which the insect sits and holds fast, while it pushes its head as far as possible into the spur, so that its proboscis may reach the nectar lying deep within it! Even though we cannot pretend to guess at the significance of every curve and colour-spot in one of the great tropical orchids, such asStanhopea tigrina, yet we may believe, with Sprengel, that all this has its significance, or has had it for the ancestors of the plant in question, and in fact that the flower is made up of nothing but adaptations, either actual or inherited from its ancestors, although sometimes perhaps no longer of functional importance.
So far, then, we have illustrated the fact that there are hundreds and thousands of contrivances in flowers adapted solely to the visits of insects and to securing cross-fertilization, and these adaptations go so far that we might almost believe them to be the outcome of the most exact calculation and the most ingenious reflection. But they all admit of interpretation through natural selection, for all these details, which used to be looked upon as merely ornamental, are directly or indirectly of use to the species; directly, when, for instance, they concern the dusting of the insect with the pollen; indirectly, when they are a means of attracting visits.
Moreover, the evidence of the operation of the processes of selection becomes absolutely convincing when we consider that, as in symbiosis, there are always two sets of adaptations taking place independently of one another—those of the flowers to the visits of the insects, and those of the insects to the habit of visiting the flowers. To understand this clearly we must turn our attention to the insects, and try to see in what way they have been changed by adapting themselves to the diet which the flowers afford.
As is well known, several orders of insects possess mouth-parts which are suited for sucking up fluids, and these have evolved, through adaptation to a fluid diet, from the biting mouth-parts of the primitive insects which we see still surviving in several orders. Thus the Diptera may have gradually acquired the sucking proboscis which occurs in many of them by licking up decaying vegetable and animal matter, and by piercing into and sucking living animals. But even among the Diptera several families have more recently adapted themselves quite specially to a flower diet, to honey-sucking, like the hover-flies, the Syrphidæ,and the Bombyliidæ, whose long thin proboscis penetrates deep into narrow corolla-tubes, and is able to suck up the nectar from the very bottom. The transformation was not so important in this case, since the already existing sucking apparatus only required to be a little altered.
Again, in the order Hemiptera (Bugs) the suctorial proboscis does not owe its origin to a diet of flowers, for no member of the group is now adapted to that mode of obtaining food.
Fig. 47.Head of a Butterfly.A, seen fromin front.au, eyes.la, upper lip.md, rudimentsof the mandibles.pm, rudimentarymaxillary palps.mx´, the first maxillæmodified into the suctorial proboscis.pl,palps of labium or second maxillæ, cut offat the root, remaining inB—which is a sideview.at, antennæ. Adapted from Savigny.
Fig. 47.Head of a Butterfly.A, seen fromin front.au, eyes.la, upper lip.md, rudimentsof the mandibles.pm, rudimentarymaxillary palps.mx´, the first maxillæmodified into the suctorial proboscis.pl,palps of labium or second maxillæ, cut offat the root, remaining inB—which is a sideview.at, antennæ. Adapted from Savigny.
The proboscis of the Lepidoptera, on the other hand, depends entirely on adaptation to honey-sucking, and we may go the length of saying that the order of Lepidoptera would not exist if there were no flowers. This large and diverse insect-group is probably descended from the ancestors of the modern caddis-flies or Phryganidæ, whose weakly developed jaws were chiefly used for licking up the sugary juices of plants. But as flowering plants evolved the licking apparatus of the primitive butterflies developed more and more into a sucking organ, and was ultimately transformed into the long, spirally coiled suctorial proboscis as we see it in the modern butterflies (Fig. 47). It has taken some pains to trace this organ back to the biting mouth-parts of the primitive insects, for nearly everything about it has degenerated and become stunted except the maxillæ (mx´). Even the palps (pm) of these have become so small and inconspicuous in most of the Lepidoptera that it is only quite recently that remains of them have been recognized in a minute protuberance among the hairs. The mandibles (md) have quite degenerated, and even the under lip has disappeared, and only its palps are well developed (B,pl). But the first maxillæ (mx´), although very strong and long, are so extraordinarily altered in shape and structure that they diverge from the maxillæ of all other insects. They have become hollow, probe-like half-tubes, which fit together exactly, and thus form a closed sucking-tube of most complex construction, composed of many very small joints, after the fashion of a chain-saw, which are all moved by little muscles, and are subject to the will through nerves, and are also furnished with tactile and taste papillæ. Except this remarkable sucking proboscis there are no peculiarities in the body of the butterfly which might beregarded as adaptations to flower-visiting, with a few isolated exceptions, of which one will be mentioned later. This is intelligible enough, for the butterfly has nothing more to seek from the flower beyond food for itself; it does not carry stores for offspring.
The bees, however, do this, and accordingly we find that in them the adaptations to flower-visiting are not confined to the mouth-parts.
As far as we can judge now, the flower-visiting bees are descended from insects which resembled the modern burrowing-wasps. Among these the females themselves live on nectar and pollen, and build cells in holes in the ground, and feed their brood. They do not feed them on honey, however, but on animals—on caterpillars, grasshoppers, and other insects, which they kill by a sting in the abdomen, or often only paralyse, so that the victim is brought into the cells of the nest alive but defenceless, and remains alive until the young larva of the wasp, which emerges from the egg, sets to work to devour it.
Fig. 48.Mouth-parts of theCockroach (Periplaneta orientalis),after R. Hertwig.la, upper lipor labrum.md, mandibles.mx1,first maxillæ, withc, cardo,st,stipes,li, internal lobe or lacinia,le, external lobe or galea, andpm,the maxillary palp.mx2, thelabium or second maxillæ, withsimilar detailed parts.
Fig. 48.Mouth-parts of theCockroach (Periplaneta orientalis),after R. Hertwig.la, upper lipor labrum.md, mandibles.mx1,first maxillæ, withc, cardo,st,stipes,li, internal lobe or lacinia,le, external lobe or galea, andpm,the maxillary palp.mx2, thelabium or second maxillæ, withsimilar detailed parts.
Before I go on to explain the origin of the sucking proboscis of the bee from the biting mouth-parts of the primitive insects I must first briefly consider the latter.
The biting mouth-parts of beetles, Neuroptera, and Orthoptera (Fig. 48), consist of three pairs of jaws, of which the first, the mandibles (md), are simply powerful pincers for seizing and tearing or chewing the food. They have no part in the development of the suctorial apparatus either in bees or in butterflies, so they may be left out of account. The two other pairs of jaws, the first and second maxillæ (mx1andmx2), are constructed exactly on the same type, having a jointed basal portion (st) bearing two lobes, an external (le) and an internal (li), and a feeler or palp, usually with several joints, directed outwards from the lobes (pmandpl). The second pair of maxillæ (mx2) differs from the first chiefly in this, that the components of the pair meet in the median line of the body, and fuse more or less to form the so-called 'under lip' or labium. In the example given, the cockroach (Periplaneta orientalis), this fusionis only partial, the lobes having remained separate (leandli); and the same is true of the bee, but in this case the inner lobes have grown into a long worm-like process which is thrust into the nectar in the act of sucking.
Fig. 49.Head of the Bee.Au, compoundeyes.au, ocelli.at, antennæ.la, upper lip.md, mandibles.mx1, first maxillæ, withpm,the rudimentary maxillary palp.mx2, secondmaxillæ with the internal lobes (li) fused toform the 'tongue.'le, the external lobes ofthe second maxillæ, known as 'paraglossæ.'pl, labial palp.
Fig. 49.Head of the Bee.Au, compoundeyes.au, ocelli.at, antennæ.la, upper lip.md, mandibles.mx1, first maxillæ, withpm,the rudimentary maxillary palp.mx2, secondmaxillæ with the internal lobes (li) fused toform the 'tongue.'le, the external lobes ofthe second maxillæ, known as 'paraglossæ.'pl, labial palp.
Even the burrowing-wasps exhibit the beginnings of variation in this direction, for the under lip is somewhat lengthened and modified into a licking organ. The adaptation has not gone much further than this, even in one of the true flower-bees,Prosopis, which feeds its larvæ with pollen and honey, and it is only in the true honey-bee that the adaptation is complete (Fig. 49). Here the so-called 'inner lobe' of the under lip (li) has elongated into the worm-shaped process already mentioned; it is thickly covered with short bristles, and is called the 'tongue' of the bee (li). The outer lobes of the under lip have degenerated into little leaf-like organs, the so-called accessory tongue or paraglossa (le), while the palps of the under lip (pl) have elongated to correspond with the tongue, and serve as a sensitive and probably also as a smelling organ, in contrast to the palps of the first maxillæ, which have shrunk to minute stumps (pm). The whole of the under lip, which has elongated even in its basal portions, forms, with the equally long first maxillæ, the proboscis of the bee. The first maxillæ are sheath-like half-tubes, closely apposed around the tongue, and form along with it the suctorial tube, through which the nectar is sucked up. Thus, of the three pairs of jaws in insects, only the first pair, the mandibles, have remained unaltered, obviously because the bee requires a biting-organ for eating pollen, for kneading wax, and for building cells.
But bees do not only feast on nectar and pollen themselves, they carry these home as food for their larvæ. The form alreadymentioned,Prosopis, takes up pollen and nectar in its mouth, and afterwards disgorges the pulp as food for its larvæ, but the rest of the true bees have special and much more effective collecting-organs, either a thick covering of hair on the abdomen, or along the whole length of the posterior legs, or finally, a highly developed collecting apparatus, such as that possessed by the honey-bee—the basket and brush on the hind leg. The former is a hollow on the outer surface of the tibia, the latter a considerable enlargement of the basal tarsal joint, which, at the same time, is covered on the inner surface with short bristles, arranged in transverse rows like a brush. The bee kneads the pollen into the basket, and one can often see bees flying back to the hive with a thick yellow ball of pollen on the hind leg. In those bees which collect on the abdomen, likeOsmiaandMegachile, the pollen mass forms a thick clump on the belly, and in the case ofAndrenaSprengel observed long ago that it sometimes flew with a packet of pollen bigger than its own body on the hind leg.
All these are contrivances which have gradually originated through the habit of carrying home pollen for the helpless larvæ shut up in the cells. They have developed differently in the various groups of bees, probably because the primary variations with which the process of selection began were different in the various ancestral forms.
In the ancestors of those which carry pollen on the abdomen there was probably a thick covering of hair on the ventral surface of the body, which served as a starting-point for the selection, and, in consequence, the further course of the adaptation would be concerned solely with this hair-covered surface, while variations in other less hairy spots would remain un-utilized.
After all this it will no longer seem a paradoxical statement that the existence of gaily coloured, diversely formed, and fragrant flowers is due to the visits of insects, and that, on the other hand, many insects have undergone essential transformations in their mouth-parts and otherwise as an adaptation to a flower diet, and that an entire order of insects with thousands of species—the Lepidoptera—would not be in existence at all if there had been no flowers. We must now attempt to show, in a more detailed way, how, by what steps, and under what conditions, our modern flowers have arisen from the earlier flowering plants. In this I follow closely the classic exposition which we owe to Hermann Müller.
The ancestral forms of the modern higher plants, the so-called 'primitive seed plants' or 'Archisperms,' were all anemophilous, as the Conifers and Cycads are still. Their smooth pollen-grains,produced in enormous quantities, fell like clouds of dust into the air, were carried by the wind hither and thither, and some occasionally alighted on the stigma of a female flower. In these plants the sexes often occur separately on different trees or individuals, and there must be a certain advantage in this when the pollination is effected by the wind.
The male flowers of the Archisperms would be visited by insects in remote ages, just as they are now; but the visitors came to feed upon the pollen, and did not render any service to the plant in return; they rather did it harm by reducing its store of pollen. If it was possible to cause the insect to benefit the plant at the same time as it was pillaging the pollen, by carrying some of it to female blossoms and thereby securing cross-fertilization, it would be of great advantage, for the plant would no longer require to produce such enormous quantities of pollen, and the fertilization would be much more certain than when it depended on the wind. It is obvious that the successful pollination of anemophilous plants implies good weather and a favourable wind.
Fig. 50.Flowers of the Willow (Salix cinerea); after H. Müller.A, the male.B, the female catkin.C, individual male flower;n, nectary.D, individual female flower;n, nectary.E, Poplar, an exceptional hermaphrodite flower.
Fig. 50.Flowers of the Willow (Salix cinerea); after H. Müller.A, the male.B, the female catkin.C, individual male flower;n, nectary.D, individual female flower;n, nectary.E, Poplar, an exceptional hermaphrodite flower.
It is plain that the utilization of the insect-visitors in fertilization might be secured in either of two ways; the female blossoms might also offer something attractive to the insects, or hermaphrodite flowers might be formed. As a matter of fact, both ways have been followed by Nature. An example of the former is the willow, the cross-fertilization of which was forced upon the insects by the development in both female and male blossoms of a nectary (Fig. 50,CandD), a little pit or basin in which nectar was secreted. The insects flew now to male and now to female willow-catkins, and in doing so theycarried to the stigma of the female blossom the pollen, which in this case was not dusty but sticky, so that it readily adhered to their bodies.
The securing of cross-fertilization by the development of hermaphrodite flowers has, however, occurred much more frequently, and we can understand that this method secured the advantageous crossing much more perfectly, for the pollen had necessarily to be carried from blossom to blossom, while, in cases like that of the willow, countless male blossoms might be visited for nectar one after the other before the insect made up its mind to fly to a female blossom of the same species. The beginnings of the modification of the unisexual flowers in this direction may be seen in variations which occur even now, for we not infrequently find, in a male catkin, individual blossoms, which, in addition to the stamens, possess also a pistil with a stigma. (Fig. 50Eshows such an abnormal hermaphrodite flower from a poplar.)
As soon as hermaphrodite flowers came into existence the struggle to attract insects began in a more intense degree. Every little improvement in this direction would form the starting-point of a process of selection, and would be carried on and increased to the highest possible pitch of perfection.
It was probably the outer envelopes of the blossoms which first changed their original green into other colours, usually those which contrasted strongly with the green, and thus directed the attention of the insects to the flowers. Variations in the colour of ordinary leaves are always cropping up from time to time, whether it be that the green is transformed into yellow or that the chlorophyll disappears more or less completely and red or blue coloured juices take its place. Many insects can undoubtedly see colour, and are attracted by the size of coloured flowers, as Hermann Müller found by counting the visits of insects to two nearly related species of mallow, one of which,Malva silvestris, has very large bright rose-red flowers visible from afar, while the other,Malva rotundifolia, has very inconspicuous small pale-red flowers. To the former there were thirty-one different visitors, to the latter he could only make sure of four. The second species, as is to be expected, depends chiefly on self-fertilization.
It has recently been disputed from various quarters that insects are attracted by the colours of the flowers, and these objections are based chiefly on experiments with artificial flowers. But when, for instance, Plateau, in the course of such experiments saw bees and butterflies first fly towards the artificial flowers, and then turn away and concern themselves no more about them, that only proves thattheir sight is sharper than we have given them credit for; for though they may be deceived at a distance, they are not so when they are near; it is possible, too, that the sense of smell turns the scale[9]. I have myself made similar experiments with diurnal butterflies, before which I placed a single artificial chrysanthemum midst a mass of natural flowers. It rarely happened indeed that a butterfly settled on the artificial flower; they usually flew first above it, but did not alight. Twice, however, I saw them alight on the artificial flower, and eagerly grope about with the proboscis for a few moments, then fly quickly away. They had visited the real chrysanthemums or horse-daisies with evident delight, and eagerly sucked up the honey from the many individual florets of every flower, and they now endeavoured to do the same in the artificial flower, and only desisted when the attempt proved unsuccessful. In this experiment the colours were of course only white and yellow; with red and blue it is probably more difficult to give the exact impression of the natural flower-colours; and in addition there is the absence of the delicate fragrance exhaled by the flower.
[9]The experiments of Plateau have since been criticized by Kienitz-Gerloff, who altogether denies their value (1903).
[9]The experiments of Plateau have since been criticized by Kienitz-Gerloff, who altogether denies their value (1903).
It must be allowed that the colour is certainly not the sole attraction to the flower; the fragrance helps in most cases, and even this is not the object of the insect's visits. The real object is the nectar, to which colour and fragrance only show the way. The development of fragrance and nectar must, like that of the colour, have been carried on and increased by processes of selection, which had their basis in the necessity for securing insect-visits, and as soon as these main qualities of the flower were established greater refinements would begin, and flower-forms would be evolved, which would diverge farther and farther, especially in shape, from the originally simple and regular form of the blossom.
The reason for this must have lain chiefly in the fact that, after insect-visits in general were secured by a flower, it would be advantageous to exclude all insects which would pillage the nectar without rendering in return the service of cross-fertilization—all those, therefore, which were unsuited either because of their minute size or because of the inconstancy of their visits. Before the butterflies and the bees existed, the regularly formed flat flower with unconcealed nectar would be visited by a mixed company of caddis-flies, saw-flies, and ichneumon-flies. But as the nectar changed its place to the deeper recesses of the flower it was withdrawn from all but the more intelligent insects, and thus the circle of visitors was already narrowedto some extent. But when in a particular species the petals fused into a short tube, all visitors were excluded whose mouth-parts were too short to reach the nectar; while among those which could reach it the process of proboscis-formation began; the under lip, or the first maxillæ, or both parts together, lengthened step for step with the corolla-tube of the flower, and thus from the caddis-flies came the butterflies, and from the ichneumon-flies the burrowing-wasps (Sphegidæ) and the bees.
At first sight one might perhaps imagine that it would have been more advantageous to the flowers to attract a great many visitors, but this is obviously not the case. On the contrary, specialized flowers, accessible only to a few visitors, have a much greater certainty of being pollinated by them, because insects which only fly to a few species are more certain to visit these, and above all to visit many flowers of the same species one after another. Hermann Müller observed that, in four minutes, one of the humming-bird hawk-moths (Macroglossa stellatarum) visited 108 different flowers of the same species, the beautiful Alpine violet (Viola calcarata), one after the other, and it may have effected an equal number of pollinations in that short time.
It was, therefore, a real advantage to the flowers to narrow their circle of visitors more and more by varying so that only the useful visitors could gain access to their nectar, and that the rest should be excluded. Thus there arose 'bee-flowers,' 'butterfly-flowers,' 'hawk-moth flowers,' and, indeed, in many cases, a species of flower has become so highly specialized that its fertilization can only be brought about by a single species of insect. This explains the remarkable adaptations of the orchids and the enormous length of the proboscis in certain butterflies. Even our own hawk-mothsMacroglossa stellatarumandSphinx convolvulishow an astonishing length of proboscis, which measures 8 cm. in the latter species. InMacrosilia cluentius, in Brazil, the proboscis is 20 cm. in length; and in Madagascar there grows an orchid with nectaries 30 cm. in length, filled with nectar to a depth of 2 cm., but the fertilizing hawk-moth is not yet known.
Thus we may say that the flowers, by varying in one direction or another, have selected a definite circle of visitors, and, conversely, that particular insect-groups have selected particular flowers for themselves, for those transformations of the flowers were always most advantageous which secured to them the exclusive visits of their best crossing agents, and these transformations were, on the one hand, such as kept off unwelcome visitors, and, on the other hand, such as attracted the most suitable ones.
From the botanical point of view the assumption that flowers and flower-visiting insects have been adapted to each other by means of processes of selection has been regarded as untenable, because every variation in the flower presupposes a corresponding one in the insect. I should not have mentioned this objection had it not come from such a famous naturalist as Nägeli, and if it were not both interesting and useful in our present discussion. Nägeli maintained that selection could not, for instance, have effected a lengthening of the corolla-tube of a flower, because the proboscis of the insects must have lengthenedsimultaneouslywith it. If the corolla-tube had lengthened alone, without the proboscis of the butterfly being at the same time elongated, the flower would no longer be fertilized at all, and if the lengthening of the proboscis preceded that of the corolla-tube it would have no value for the butterfly, and could not therefore have been the object of a process of selection.
This objection overlooks the facts that a species of plant and of butterfly consists not of one individual but of thousands or millions, and that these are not absolutely uniform, but in fact heterogeneous. It is precisely in this that the struggle for existence consists—that the individuals of every species differ from one another, and that some are better, others less well constituted. The elimination of the latter and the preferring of the former constitutes the process of selection, which always secures the fitter by continually rejecting the less fit. In the case we are considering, then, there would be, among the individuals of the plant-species concerned, flowers with a longer and flowers with a shorter corolla-tube, and among the butterflies some with a longer and some with a shorter proboscis. If among the flowers the longer ones were more certain to be cross-fertilized than the shorter ones, because hurtful visitors were better excluded, the longer ones would produce more and better seeds, and would transmit their character to more descendants; and if, among the butterflies, those with the longer proboscis had an advantage, because the nectar in the longer tubes would, so to speak, be reserved for them, and they would thus be better nourished than those with the shorter proboscis, the number of individuals with long proboscis must have increased from generation to generation. Thus the length of the corolla-tube and the length of the proboscis would go on increasing as long as there was any advantage in it for the flower, and both parties must of necessity have variedpari passu, since every lengthening of the corolla was accompanied by a preferring of the longest proboscis variation. The augmentation of the characters depended on, and could only have depended on, a guiding of the variations in thedirection of utility. But this is exactly what we call, after Darwin and Wallace, Natural Selection.
We have, however, in the history of flowers, a means of demonstrating the reality of the processes of selection in two other ways. In the first place, it is obvious that no other interpretation can be given of such simultaneous mutual adaptations of two different kinds of organisms. If we were to postulate, as Nägeli, for instance, did, an intrinsic Power of Development in organisms, which produces and guides their variations, we should, as I have already said, be compelled also to take for granted a kind of pre-established harmony, such as Leibnitz assumed to account for the correlation of body and mind: plant and insect must always have been correspondingly altered so that they bore the same relation to each other as two clocks which were so exactly fashioned that they always kept time, though they did not influence each other. But the case would be more complicated than that of the clocks, because the changes which must have taken place on both sides were quite different, and yet at the same time such that they corresponded as exactly as Will and Action. The whole history of the earth and of the forms of life must, therefore, have been foreseen down to the smallest details, and embodied in the postulated Power of Development.
But such an assumption could hardly lay claim to the rank of a scientific hypothesis. Although every grain of sand blown about by the wind on this earth could certainly only have fallen where it actually did fall, yet it is in the power of any of us to throw a handful of sand wherever it pleases us, and although even this act of throwing must have had its sufficient reason in us, yet no one could maintain that its direction and the places where the grains fell were predestined in the history of the earth. In other words: That which we call chance plays a part also in the evolution of organisms, and the assumption of a Power of Development, predestinating even in detail, is contradicted by the fact that species are transformed in accordance with the chance conditions of their life.
This can be clearly demonstrated in the case of flowers. That the wild pansy (Viola tricolor), which lives in the plains and on mountains of moderate elevation, is fertilized by bees, and the nearly alliedViola calcarataof the High Alps by Lepidoptera, is readily intelligible, since bees are very abundant in the lower region, and make the fertilization of the species a certainty, while this is not so in the High Alps. There the Lepidoptera are greatly in the majority, as every one knows who has traversed the flower-decked meads of the High Alps in July, and has seen the hundreds and thousandsof butterflies and moths which fly from flower to flower. Thus the viola of the High Alps has become a 'butterfly-flower' by the development of its nectaries into a long spur, accessible only to the proboscis of a moth or butterfly. The chance which led certain individuals of the ancestral species to climb the Alps must also have supplied the incentive to the production of the changes adapted to the visits of the prevalent insect. The hypothesis of a predestinating Power of Development suffers utter shipwreck in face of facts like these.
We have, furthermore, an excellent touchstone for the reality of the processes of selection in thequalityof the variations in flowers and insects. Natural selection can only bring about those changes which are of use to the possessors themselves; we should therefore expect to find among flowers only such arrangements as are, directly or indirectly, of use to them, and, conversely, among insects only such as are useful to the insect.
And this is what we actually do find. All the arrangements of the flowers—their colour, their form, their honey-guides, their hairy honey-paths (Iris), their fragrance, and their honey itself—are all indirectly useful to the plant itself, because they all co-operate in compelling the honey-seeking insect to effect the fertilization of the flower. This is most clearly seen in the case of the so-called 'Deceptive' flowers, which attract insects by their size and beauty, their fragrance, and their resemblance to other flowers, and force their visitors to be the means of their cross-fertilization, although they contain no nectar at all. This is the case, according to Hermann Müller, with the most beautiful of our indigenous orchids, the lady's slipper (Cypripedium calceolaris). This flower is visited by bees of the genusAndrena, which creep into the large wooden-shoe-shaped under lip in the search for honey, only to find themselves prisoners, for they cannot get out, at least by the way they came in, because of the steep and smoothly polished walls of the flower. There is only one way for the bee; it must force itself under the stigma, which it can only do with great exertion, and not without being smeared with pollen, which it carries to the next flower into which it creeps. It can only leave this one in the same way, and thus the pollen is transferred to the stigma by a mechanical necessity.
Such remarkable cases remind us in some ways of those cases of mimicry in which the deceptions have to be used with caution or they lose their effect. One might be disposed to imagine that such an intelligent insect as a bee would not be deceived by the lady's slipper more than once, and would not creep into a second flower after discovering that there was no nectar in the first. But thisconclusion is not correct, for the bees are well accustomed in many flowers to find that the nectar has already been taken by other bees; they could therefore not conclude from one unsuccessful visit that theCypripediumdid not produce nectar at all, but would try again in a second, a third, and a fourth flower. If these orchids had abundantly covered flower-spikes like many species ofOrchis, and if the species were common, the bees would probably soon learn not to visit them, but the reverse is the case. There is usually only one or, at most, two open flowers on the lady's slipper, and the plant is rare, and probably occurs nowhere in large numbers.
If we could find a flower in which the nectar lay open and accessible to all insects, and which did not require any service from them in return, the case could not be interpreted in terms of natural selection; but we do not know of any such case.