FOOTNOTES

AmountPercentage of Correct FiguresInitial ScoreGainInitial ScoreGainInitially highest five individuals85617018next  five     "56516810next   six     "4622748next   six     "3885812next   six     "29245614

Achievement in arithmetic depends upon a number of different abilities. For example, accuracy in copying numbers depends upon eyesight, ability to perceive visual details, and short-term memory for these. Long column addition depends chiefly upon great strength of the addition combinations especially in higher decades, 'carrying,' and keeping one's place in the column. The solution of problems framed in words requires understanding of language, the analysis of the situation described into its elements, the selection of the right elements for use at each step and their use in the right relations.

Since the abilities which together constitute arithmetic ability are thus specialized, the individual who is the best of a thousand of his age or grade in respect to, say, adding integers, may occupy different stations, perhaps from 1st to 600th, in multiplying with integers, placing the decimal point in division with decimals, solving novel problems, copying figures, etc., etc. Such specialization is in part due to his having had, relatively to the others in the thousand, more or better training in certain of these abilities than in others, and to various circumstances of life which have caused him to have, relatively to the others in the thousand, greater interest in certain of these achievements than in others. The specialization is not wholly due thereto, however. Certain inborn characteristics of an individual predispose him to different degrees of superiority or inferiority to other men in different features of arithmetic.

We measure the extent to which ability of one sort goes with or fails to go with ability of some other sort by the coefficient of correlation between the two. If every individual keeps the same rank in the second ability—if the individual who is the best of the thousand in one is the best of the group in the other, and so on down the list—the correlation is 1.00. In proportion as the ranks of individuals vary in the two abilities the coefficient drops from 1.00, a coefficient of 0 meaning that the best individual in ability A is no more likely to be in first place in ability B than to be in any other rank.

The meanings of coefficients of correlation of .90, .70, .50, and 0 are shown by Tables 15, 16, 17 and 18.[26]

TABLE 15

Distribution of Arrays in Successive Tenths of the Group Whenr= .90

10TH9TH8TH7TH6TH5TH4TH3D2D1ST1st tenth.1.41.86.622.468.72d tenth.1.41.44.711.523.536.022.43d tenth.1.52.15.812.821.127.423.56.64th tenth.42.16.412.820.123.821.211.51.85th tenth.11.45.812.819.322.620.112.84.7.46th tenth.44.712.820.122.619.312.85.81.4.17th tenth1.811.521.223.820.112.86.42.1.48th tenth6.623.527.421.112.85.82.1.5.19th tenth22.436.023.511.54.71.4.4.110th tenth68.722.46.61.8.4.1

TABLE 16

Distribution of Arrays in Successive Tenths of the Group Whenr= .70

10TH9TH8TH7TH6TH5TH4TH3D2D1ST1st tenth.2.71.52.84.88.013.022.346.72d tenth.21.22.64.57.09.813.417.321.722.33d tenth.72.65.07.310.012.514.916.717.313.04th tenth1.54.57.39.812.013.714.814.913.48.05th tenth2.87.010.012.013.414.013.712.59.84.86th tenth4.89.812.513.714.013.412.010.07.02.87th tenth8.013.414.914.813.712.09.87.34.51.58th tenth13.017.316.714.912.510.07.35.02.6.79th tenth22.321.717.313.49.87.04.52.61.2.210th tenth46.722.313.08.04.82.81.5.7.2

TABLE 17

Distribution of Arrays of Successive Tenths of the Group Whenr= .50

10TH9TH8TH7TH6TH5TH4TH3D2D1ST1st tenth.82.03.24.66.28.110.513.918.031.82d tenth2.04.15.77.38.810.512.214.116.418.93d tenth3.25.77.48.910.011.212.313.314.113.94th tenth4.67.38.89.910.811.612.012.312.210.55th tenth6.28.810.010.811.311.511.611.210.58.16th tenth8.110.511.211.611.511.310.810.08.86.27th tenth10.512.212.312.011.610.89.98.87.54.68th tenth13.914.113.312.311.210.08.87.45.73.29th tenth18.916.414.112.210.58.87.35.74.12.010th tenth31.818.913.910.58.16.24.63.22.0.8

TABLE 18

Distribution of Arrays, in Successive Tenths of the Group Whenr= .0

10TH9TH8TH7TH6TH5TH4TH3D2D1ST1st tenth101010101010101010102d tenth101010101010101010103d tenth101010101010101010104th tenth101010101010101010105th tenth101010101010101010106th tenth101010101010101010107th tenth101010101010101010108th tenth101010101010101010109th tenth1010101010101010101010th tenth10101010101010101010

The significance of any coefficient of correlation depends upon the group of individuals for which it is determined. A correlation of .40 between computation and problem-solvingin eighth-grade pupils of 14 years would mean a much closer real relation than a correlation of .40 in all 14-year-olds, and a very, very much closer relation than a correlation of .40 for all children 8 to 15.

Unless the individuals concerned are very elaborately tested on several days, the correlations obtained are "attenuated" toward 0 by the "accidental" errors in the original measurements. This effect was not known until 1904; consequently the correlations in the earlier studies of arithmetic are all too low.

In general, the correlation between ability in any one important feature of computation and ability in any other important feature of computation is high. If we make enough tests to measure each individual exactly in:—

(A) Subtraction with integers and decimals,

(B) Multiplication with integers and decimals,

(C) Division with integers and decimals,

(D) Multiplication and division with common fractions, and

(E) Computing with percents,

we shall probably find the intercorrelations for a thousand 14-year-olds to be near .90. Addition of integers (F) will, however, correlate less closely with any of the above, being apparently dependent on simpler and more isolated abilities.

The correlation between problem-solving (G) and computation will be very much less, probably not over .60.

It should be noted that even when the correlation is as high as .90, there will be some individuals very high in one ability and very low in the other. Such disparities are to some extent, as Courtis ['13, pp. 67-75] and Cobb ['17] have argued, due to inborn characteristics of the individual in question which predispose him to very special sorts ofstrength and weakness. They are often due, however, to defects in his learning whereby he has acquired more ability than he needs in one line of work or has failed to acquire some needed ability which was well within his capacity.

In general, all correlations between an individual's divergence from the common type or average of his age for one arithmetical function, and his divergences from the average for any other arithmetical function, are positive. The correlation due to original capacity more than counterbalances the effects that robbing Peter to pay Paul may have.

Speed and accuracy are thus positively correlated. The individuals who do the most work in ten minutes will be above the average in a test of accuracy. The common notion that speed is opposed to accuracy is correct when it means that the same person will tend to make more errors if he works at too rapid a rate; but it is entirely wrong when it means that the kind of person who works more rapidly than the average person is likely to be less accurate than the average person.

Interest in arithmetic and ability at arithmetic are probably correlated positively in the sense that the pupil who has more interest than other pupils of his age tends in the long run to have more ability than they. They are certainly correlated in the sense that the pupil who 'likes' arithmetic better than geography or history tends to have relatively more ability in arithmetic, or, in other words, that the pupil who is more gifted at arithmetic than at drawing or English tends also to like it better than he likes these. These correlations are high.

It is correct then to think of mathematical ability as, in a sense, a unitary ability of which any one individual may have much or little, most individuals possessing a moderateamount of it. This is consistent, however, with the occasional appearance of individuals possessed of very great talents for this or that particular feature of mathematical ability and equally notable deficiencies in other features.

Finally it may be noted that ability in arithmetic, though occasionally found in men otherwise very stupid, is usually associated with superior intelligence in dealing with ideas and symbols of all sorts, and is one of the best early indications thereof.

FOOTNOTES[1]The following and later problems are taken from actual textbooks or courses of study or state examinations; to avoid invidious comparisons, they are not exact quotations, but are equivalents in principle and form, as stated in the preface.[2]The work of Mitchell has not been published, but the author has had the privilege of examining it.[3]The form of Test 6 quoted here is that given by Courtis ['11-'12, p. 20]. This differs a little from the other series of Test 6, shown on pages 43 and 44.[4]Eight or ten timesin all, not eight or ten times for each fact of the tables.[5]The facts concerning the present inaccuracy of school work in arithmetic will be found on pages 102 to 105.[6]McLellan and Ames,Public School Arithmetic[1900].[7]These concern allowances for two errors occurring in the same example and for the same wrong answer being obtained in both original work and check work.[8]The very early learning of 2 × 2, 2 × 3, 3 × 2, 2 × 4, 4 × 2, 3 × 3, and perhaps a few more multiplications is not considered here. It is advisable. The treatment of 0 × 0, 0 × 1, 1 × 0, etc., is not considered here. It is probably best to defer the '× 0' bonds until after all the others are formed and are being used in short multiplication, and to form them in close connection with their use in short multiplication. The '0 ×' bonds may well be deferred until they are needed in 'long' multiplication, 0 × 0 coming last of all.[9]See page 76.[10]At the end of a volume or part, the count may be from as few as 5 or as many as 12 pages.[11]Certain paragraphs in this and the following chapter are taken from the author'sEducational Psychology, with slight modifications.[12]It should be noted that just as concretes give rise to abstractions, so these in turn give rise to still more abstract abstractions. Thus fourness, fiveness, twentyness, and the like give rise to 'integral-number-ness.' Similarly just as individuals are grouped into general classes, so classes are grouped into still more general classes. Half, quarter, sixth, and tenth are general notions, but 'one ...th' is more general; and 'fraction' is still more general.[13]They may, of course, also result in a fusion or an alternation of responses, but only rarely.[14]The more gifted children may be put to work using the principle after the first minute or two.[15]If desired this form may be used, with the appropriate difference in the form of the questions and statements.232300006966960[16]Courtis finds in the case of addition that "of all the individuals making mistakes at any given time in a class, at least one third, and usually two thirds, will be making mistakes in carrying or copying."[17]Facts concerning the conditions of learning in general will be found in the author'sEducational Psychology, Vol. 2, Chapter 8, or in theEducational Psychology, Briefer Course, Chapter 15.[18]See Thorndike ['00], King ['07], and Heck ['13].[19]A special type could be constructed that would use a large type body, say 14 point, with integers in 10 or 12 point and fractions much larger than now.[20]It will be still better if the 4 is replaced by an open-top 4.[21]For an account in English of their main findings see Howell ['14], pp. 149-251.[22]In hisHow We Think.[23]Compiled from data on p. 89 of Kruse ['18].[24]Siblings is used for children of the same parents.[25]Similar results have been obtained in the case of arithmetical and other abilities by Thorndike ['08, '10, '15, '16], Whitley ['11], Starch ['11], Wells ['12], Kirby ['13], Donovan and Thorndike ['13], Hahn and Thorndike ['14], and on a very large scale by Race in a study as yet unpublished.[26]Unless he has a thorough understanding of the underlying theory, the student should be very cautious in making inferences from coefficients of correlation.

[1]The following and later problems are taken from actual textbooks or courses of study or state examinations; to avoid invidious comparisons, they are not exact quotations, but are equivalents in principle and form, as stated in the preface.

[1]The following and later problems are taken from actual textbooks or courses of study or state examinations; to avoid invidious comparisons, they are not exact quotations, but are equivalents in principle and form, as stated in the preface.

[2]The work of Mitchell has not been published, but the author has had the privilege of examining it.

[2]The work of Mitchell has not been published, but the author has had the privilege of examining it.

[3]The form of Test 6 quoted here is that given by Courtis ['11-'12, p. 20]. This differs a little from the other series of Test 6, shown on pages 43 and 44.

[3]The form of Test 6 quoted here is that given by Courtis ['11-'12, p. 20]. This differs a little from the other series of Test 6, shown on pages 43 and 44.

[4]Eight or ten timesin all, not eight or ten times for each fact of the tables.

[4]Eight or ten timesin all, not eight or ten times for each fact of the tables.

[5]The facts concerning the present inaccuracy of school work in arithmetic will be found on pages 102 to 105.

[5]The facts concerning the present inaccuracy of school work in arithmetic will be found on pages 102 to 105.

[6]McLellan and Ames,Public School Arithmetic[1900].

[6]McLellan and Ames,Public School Arithmetic[1900].

[7]These concern allowances for two errors occurring in the same example and for the same wrong answer being obtained in both original work and check work.

[7]These concern allowances for two errors occurring in the same example and for the same wrong answer being obtained in both original work and check work.

[8]The very early learning of 2 × 2, 2 × 3, 3 × 2, 2 × 4, 4 × 2, 3 × 3, and perhaps a few more multiplications is not considered here. It is advisable. The treatment of 0 × 0, 0 × 1, 1 × 0, etc., is not considered here. It is probably best to defer the '× 0' bonds until after all the others are formed and are being used in short multiplication, and to form them in close connection with their use in short multiplication. The '0 ×' bonds may well be deferred until they are needed in 'long' multiplication, 0 × 0 coming last of all.

[8]The very early learning of 2 × 2, 2 × 3, 3 × 2, 2 × 4, 4 × 2, 3 × 3, and perhaps a few more multiplications is not considered here. It is advisable. The treatment of 0 × 0, 0 × 1, 1 × 0, etc., is not considered here. It is probably best to defer the '× 0' bonds until after all the others are formed and are being used in short multiplication, and to form them in close connection with their use in short multiplication. The '0 ×' bonds may well be deferred until they are needed in 'long' multiplication, 0 × 0 coming last of all.

[9]See page 76.

[9]See page 76.

[10]At the end of a volume or part, the count may be from as few as 5 or as many as 12 pages.

[10]At the end of a volume or part, the count may be from as few as 5 or as many as 12 pages.

[11]Certain paragraphs in this and the following chapter are taken from the author'sEducational Psychology, with slight modifications.

[11]Certain paragraphs in this and the following chapter are taken from the author'sEducational Psychology, with slight modifications.

[12]It should be noted that just as concretes give rise to abstractions, so these in turn give rise to still more abstract abstractions. Thus fourness, fiveness, twentyness, and the like give rise to 'integral-number-ness.' Similarly just as individuals are grouped into general classes, so classes are grouped into still more general classes. Half, quarter, sixth, and tenth are general notions, but 'one ...th' is more general; and 'fraction' is still more general.

[12]It should be noted that just as concretes give rise to abstractions, so these in turn give rise to still more abstract abstractions. Thus fourness, fiveness, twentyness, and the like give rise to 'integral-number-ness.' Similarly just as individuals are grouped into general classes, so classes are grouped into still more general classes. Half, quarter, sixth, and tenth are general notions, but 'one ...th' is more general; and 'fraction' is still more general.

[13]They may, of course, also result in a fusion or an alternation of responses, but only rarely.

[13]They may, of course, also result in a fusion or an alternation of responses, but only rarely.

[14]The more gifted children may be put to work using the principle after the first minute or two.

[14]The more gifted children may be put to work using the principle after the first minute or two.

[15]If desired this form may be used, with the appropriate difference in the form of the questions and statements.232300006966960

[15]If desired this form may be used, with the appropriate difference in the form of the questions and statements.

[16]Courtis finds in the case of addition that "of all the individuals making mistakes at any given time in a class, at least one third, and usually two thirds, will be making mistakes in carrying or copying."

[16]Courtis finds in the case of addition that "of all the individuals making mistakes at any given time in a class, at least one third, and usually two thirds, will be making mistakes in carrying or copying."

[17]Facts concerning the conditions of learning in general will be found in the author'sEducational Psychology, Vol. 2, Chapter 8, or in theEducational Psychology, Briefer Course, Chapter 15.

[17]Facts concerning the conditions of learning in general will be found in the author'sEducational Psychology, Vol. 2, Chapter 8, or in theEducational Psychology, Briefer Course, Chapter 15.

[18]See Thorndike ['00], King ['07], and Heck ['13].

[18]See Thorndike ['00], King ['07], and Heck ['13].

[19]A special type could be constructed that would use a large type body, say 14 point, with integers in 10 or 12 point and fractions much larger than now.

[19]A special type could be constructed that would use a large type body, say 14 point, with integers in 10 or 12 point and fractions much larger than now.

[20]It will be still better if the 4 is replaced by an open-top 4.

[20]It will be still better if the 4 is replaced by an open-top 4.

[21]For an account in English of their main findings see Howell ['14], pp. 149-251.

[21]For an account in English of their main findings see Howell ['14], pp. 149-251.

[22]In hisHow We Think.

[22]In hisHow We Think.

[23]Compiled from data on p. 89 of Kruse ['18].

[23]Compiled from data on p. 89 of Kruse ['18].

[24]Siblings is used for children of the same parents.

[24]Siblings is used for children of the same parents.

[25]Similar results have been obtained in the case of arithmetical and other abilities by Thorndike ['08, '10, '15, '16], Whitley ['11], Starch ['11], Wells ['12], Kirby ['13], Donovan and Thorndike ['13], Hahn and Thorndike ['14], and on a very large scale by Race in a study as yet unpublished.

[25]Similar results have been obtained in the case of arithmetical and other abilities by Thorndike ['08, '10, '15, '16], Whitley ['11], Starch ['11], Wells ['12], Kirby ['13], Donovan and Thorndike ['13], Hahn and Thorndike ['14], and on a very large scale by Race in a study as yet unpublished.

[26]Unless he has a thorough understanding of the underlying theory, the student should be very cautious in making inferences from coefficients of correlation.

[26]Unless he has a thorough understanding of the underlying theory, the student should be very cautious in making inferences from coefficients of correlation.

Ames, A. F., and McLellan, J. F.'00Public School Arithmetic.Ballou, F. W.'16Determining the Achievements of Pupils in the Addition of Fractions. School Document No. 3, 1916, Boston Public Schools.Brandell, G.'13Skolbarns intressen. Translated ['15] by W. Stern as, Das Interesse der Schulkinder an den Unterrichtsfächern.Brandford, B.'08A Study of Mathematical Education.Brown, J. C.'11, '12An Investigation on the Value of Drill Work in the Fundamental Operations in Arithmetic. Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 2, pp. 81-88, vol. 3, pp. 485-492 and 561-570.Brown, J. C. and Coffman, L. D.'14How to Teach Arithmetic.Burgerstein, L.'91Die Arbeitscurve einer Schulstunde. Zeitschrift für Schulgesundheitspflege, vol. 4, pp. 543-562 and 607-627.Burnett, C. J.'06The Estimation of Number. Harvard Psychological Studies, vol. 2, pp. 349-404.Burt, C.'17The Distribution and Relations of Educational Abilities. Report of The London County Council, No. 1868.Chapman, J. C.'14Individual Differences in Ability and Improvement and Their Correlations. Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 63.Chapman, J. C.'17The Scientific Measurement of Classroom Products. (With G. P. Rush.)Cobb, M. V.'17A Preliminary Study of the Inheritance of Arithmetical Abilities. Jour. of Educational Psychology, vol. 8, pp. 1-20. Jan., 1917.Coffman, L. D., and Brown, J. C.'14How to Teach Arithmetic.Coffman, L. D., and Jessup, W. A.'16The Supervision of Arithmetic.Courtis, S. A.'09, '10, '11Measurement of Growth and Efficiency in Arithmetic. Elementary School Teacher, vol. 10, pp. 58-74 and 177-199, vol. 11, pp. 171-185, 360-370, and 528-539.Courtis, S. A.'11, '12Report on Educational Aspects of the Public School System of the City of New York. Part II, Subdivision 1, Section D. Report on the Courtis Tests in Arithmetic.Courtis, S. A.'13Courtis Standard Tests. Second Annual Accounting.Courtis, S. A.'14Manual of Instructions for Giving and Scoring the Courtis StandardTests in the Three R's. Department of Comparative Research, 82 Elliot St., Detroit, Mich., 1914.Decroly, M., and Degand, J.'12L'evolution des notions de quantités continues et discontinues chez l'enfant. Archives de psychologie, vol. 12, pp. 81-121.Degand, J.SeeDecroly.De Voss, J. C.SeeMonroe, De Voss, and Kelly.Dewey, J.'10How We Think.Dewey, J., and McLellan, J. A.'95Psychology of Number and Its Applications to Methods of Teaching Arithmetic.Donovan, M. E., and Thorndike, E. L.'13Improvement in a Practice Experiment under School Conditions. American Journal of Psychology, vol. 24, pp. 426-428.Elliott, C. H.'14Variation in the Achievements of Pupils. Teachers College, Columbia University, Contributions to Education, No. 72.Flynn, F. J.'12Mathematical Games—Adaptations from Games Old and New. Teachers College Record, vol. 13, pp. 399-412.Freeman, F. N.'10Untersuchungen über den Aufmerksamkeitsumfang und die Zahlauffassung. Pädagogische-Psychologische Arbeiten, I, 88-168.Friedrich, J.'97Untersuchungen über die Einflüsse der Arbeitsdauer und die Arbeitspausen auf die geistige Leistungsfähigkeit der Schulkinder. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, vol. 13, pp. 1-53.Gilbert, J. A.'94Researches on the Mental andPhysical Development of School Children. Studies from the Yale Psychological Laboratory, vol. 2, pp. 40-100.Greenleaf, B.'73Practical Arithmetic.Hahn, H. H., and Thorndike, E. L.'14Some Results of Practice in Addition under School Conditions. Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 5, No. 2, pp. 65-84.Hall, G. S.'83The Contents of Children's Minds on Entering School. Princeton Review, vol. II, pp. 249-272, May, 1883. Reprinted in Aspects of Child Life and Education, 1907.Hartmann, B.'90Die Analyze des Kindlichen Gedanken-Kreises als die Naturgemässedes Ersten Schulunterrichts, 1890.Heck, W. H.'13A Study of Mental Fatigue.Heck, W. H.'13A Second Study in Mental Fatigue in the Daily School Program. Psychological Clinic, vol. 7, pp. 29-34.Hoffmann, P.'11Das Interesse der Schüler an den Unterrichtsfächern. Zeitschrift für pädagogische Psychologie, XII, 458-470.Hoke, K. J., and Wilson, G. M.'20How to Measure.Holmes, M. E.'95The Fatigue of a School Hour. Pedagogical Seminary, vol. 3, pp. 213-234.Howell, H. B.'14A Foundation Study in the Pedagogy of Arithmetic.Hunt, C. W.'12Play and Recreation in Arithmetic. Teachers College Record, vol. 13, pp. 388-398.Jessup, W. A., and Coffman, L. D.'16The Supervision of Arithmetic.Kelly, F. J.SeeMonroe, De Voss and Kelly.King, A. C.'07The Daily Program in Elementary Schools. MSS.Kirby, T. J.'13Practice in the Case of School Children. Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 58.Klapper, P.'16The Teaching of Arithmetic.Kruse, P. J.'18The Overlapping of Attainments in Certain Sixth, Seventh, and Eighth Grades. Teachers College, Columbia University, Contributions to Education, No. 92.Laser, H.'94Ueber geistige Ermüdung beim Schulunterricht. Zeitschrift für Schulgesundheitspflege, vol. 7, pp. 2-22.Lay, W. A.'98Führer durch den ersten Rechenunterricht.Lay, W. A.'07Führer durch den Rechenunterricht der Unterstufe.Lewis, E. O.'13Popular and Unpopular School-Subjects. The Journal of Experimental Pedagogy, vol. 2, pp. 89-98.Lobsien, M.'03Kinderideale. Zeitschrift für pädagogische Psychologie, V, 323-344 and 457-494.Lobsien, M.'09Beliebtheit und Unbeliebtheit der Unterrichtsfächer. Pädagogisches Magazin, Heft 361.McCall, W. A.'21How to Measure in Education.McDougle, E. C.'14A Contribution to the Pedagogy of Arithmetic. Pedagogical Seminary, vol. 21, pp. 161-218.McKnight, J.A.'07Differentiation of the Curriculum in the Upper Grammar Grades. MSS. in the library of Teachers College, Columbia University.McLellan, J.A., and Dewey, J.'95Psychology of Number and Its Applications to Methods of Teaching.McLellan, J.A., and Ames, A.F.'00Public School Arithmetic.Messenger, J.F.'03The Perception of Number. Psychological Review, Monograph Supplement No. 22.Meumann, E.'07Vorlesungen zur Einführung in die experimentelle Pädagogik.Mitchell, H.E.'20Unpublished studies of the uses of arithmetic in factories, shops, farms, and the like.Monroe, W.S., De Voss, J.C., and Kelly, F.J.'17Educational Tests and Measurements.Nanu, H.A.'04Zur Psychologie der Zahl Auffassung.National Intelligence Tests'20Scale A, Form 1, Edition 1.Phillips, D.E.'97Number and Its Application Psychologically Considered. Pedagogical Seminary, vol. 5, pp. 221-281.Pommer, O.'14Die Erforschung der Beliebtheit der Unterrichtsfächer. Ihre psychologischen Grundlagen und ihre pädagog. Bedeutung. VII. Jahresber. des k.k. Ssaatsgymn. im XVIII Bez. v. Wien.Rice, J.M.'02Test in Arithmetic. Forum, vol. 34, pp. 281-297.Rice, J.M.'03Causes of Success and Failure in Arithmetic. Forum, vol. 34, pp. 437-452.Rush, G.P.'17The Scientific Measurement of Classroom Products. (With J. C. Chapman.)Seekel, E.'14Ueber die Beziehung zwischen der Beliebtheit und der Schwierigkeit der Schulfächer. Ergebnisse einer Erhebung. Zeitschrift für Angewandte Psychologie 9. S. 268-277.Selkin, F. B.'12Number Games Bordering on Arithmetic and Algebra. Teachers College Record, vol. 13, pp. 452-493.Smith, D. E.'01The Teaching of Elementary Mathematics.Smith, D. E.'11The Teaching of Arithmetic.Speer, W. W.'97Arithmetic: Elementary for Pupils.Starch, D.'11Transfer of Training in Arithmetical Operations. Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 2, pp. 306-310.Starch, D.'16Educational Measurements.Stern, W.'05Ueber Beliebtheit und Unbeliebtheit der Schulfächer. Zeitschrift für pädagogische Psychologie, VII, 267-296.Stern, C., and Stern, W.'13Beliebtheit und Schwierigkeit der Schulfächer. (Freie Schulgemeinde Wickersdorf.) Auf Grund der von Herrn Luserke beschafften Materialien bearbeitet. In: "Die Ausstellung zur vergleichenden Jungendkunde der Geschlechter in Breslau." Arbeit 7 des Bundes für Schulreform. S. 24-26.Stern, W.'14Zur vergleichenden Jugendkunde der Geschlechter. Vortrag. III. Deutsch. Kongr. f. Jugendkundeusw. Arbeiten 8 des Bundes für Schulreform. S. 17-38.Stone, C.W.'08Arithmetical Abilities and Some Factors Determining Them. Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 19.Suzzallo, H.'11The Teaching of Primary Arithmetic.Thorndike, E.L.'00Mental Fatigue. Psychological Review, vol. 7, pp. 466-482 and 547-579.Thorndike, E.L.'08The Effect of Practice in the Case of a Purely Intellectual Function. American Journal of Psychology, vol. 10, pp. 374-384.Thorndike, E.L.'10Practice in the case of Addition. American Journal of Psychology, vol. 21, pp. 483-486.Thorndike, E.L., and Donovan, M.E.'13Improvement in a Practice Experiment under School Conditions. American Journal of Psychology, vol. 24, pp. 426-428.Thorndike, E.L., and Donovan, M.E., and Hahn, H.H.'14Some Results of Practice in Addition under School Conditions. Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 5, No. 2, pp. 65-84.Thorndike, E.L.'15The Relation between Initial Ability and Improvement in a Substitution Test. School and Society, vol. 12, p. 429.Thorndike, E.L.'16Notes on Practice, Improvability and the Curve of Work. American Journal of Psychology, vol 27, pp. 550-565.Walsh, J.H.'06Grammar School Arithmetic.Wells, F.L.'12The Relation of Practice to Individual Differences.American Journal of Psychology, vol. 23, pp. 75-88.White, E. E.'83A New Elementary Arithmetic.Whitley, M. T.'11An Empirical Study of Certain Tests for Individual Differences. Archives of Psychology, No. 19.Wiederkehr, G.'07Statistiche Untersuchungen über die Art und den Grad des Interesses bei Kindern der Volksschule. Neue Bahnen, vol. 19, pp. 241-251, 289-299.Wilson, G. M.'19A Survey of the Social and Business Usage of Arithmetic. Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 100.Wilson, G. M., and Hoke, K. J.'20How to Measure.Woody, C.'10Measurements of Some Achievements in Arithmetic. Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 80.


Back to IndexNext